Political Culture versus Rational Choice: Explaining Responses to Transition in the Czech Republic and Slovakia Author(s): Stephen Whitefield and Geoffrey Evans Source: British Journal of Political Science, Vol. 29, No. 1 (Jan., 1999), pp. 129-154 Published by: Cambridge University Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/194299 Accessed: 20-02-2017 13:01 UTC JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms Cambridge University Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to British Journal of Political Science This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms B. J. Pol. S. 29, 129-155 Copyright ? 1999 Cambridge University Press Printed in Great Britain Political Culture Versus Rational Choice: Explaining Responses to Transition in the Czech Republic and Slovakia STEPHEN WHITEFIELD AND GEOFFREY EVANS* There has been considerable disagreement among political scientists over t political culture versus rational choice explanations of democratic and commitments. However, empirical tests of their relative explanatory pow evidence have been in short supply. This article employs national probabil conducted in 1994 to assess differences between Czechs and Slovaks in democratic norms and liberal attitudes with respect to economic, politi issues. The applicability of an explanation focusing on long-standing cultura the two countries is compared with a rational choice explanation based on in their recent experiences of political and economic transition. It is shown the expression of support for marketization and democracy in the two countr relatively parsimoniously in rational choice terms. The explanatory con culture appears to relate only to a narrow range of attitudes and values. The debate between 'political cultural' and 'rationalist' exp Eckstein among others has argued, is central to contemporary and to an understanding of political phenomena.l However fundamental and competing perspectives are not easy to f literature.2 The aim of this article is to take up the emp evaluating the value of political cultural versus rationalis context which arguably affords a good test of the exten usefulness - the case of the Czech Republic and Slovakia which theory offers the most appropriate and efficient expl on citizens' orientations towards liberalization. The term 'political culture' is famously (or notoriously) problematic even within the camp of scholars who find the term useful. There is little agreement among its practitioners on its scope and meaning, on how it may be measured, * Pembroke College, Oxford; and Nuffield College, Oxford, respectively. Responsibility for this article is held equally by the two authors. The research reported here was commissioned as part of the Economic and Social Research Council's East-West Programme, phase II, Grant no. Y309253025 'Emerging Forms of Political Representation and Participation in Eastern Europe'. The authors would like to thank Karen Henderson, Kieran Williams, Albert Weale and the Journal's three reviewers for comments on earlier drafts of this article. Harry Eckstein, 'A Culturalist Theory of Political Change', American Political Science Review, 82 (1988), 789-804. 2 An exception is Mitchell A. Seligson and John A. Booth, 'Political Culture and Regime Type: Evidence from Nicaragua and Costa Rica', Journal of Politics, 55 (1993), 777-92. B. J. Pol. S. 29, 129-155 Copyright ? 1999 Cambridge University Press Printed in Great Britain Political Culture Versus Rational Choice: Explaining Responses to Transition in the Czech Republic and Slovakia STEPHEN WHITEFIELD AND GEOFFREY EVANS* There has been considerable disagreement among political scientists over t political culture versus rational choice explanations of democratic and commitments. However, empirical tests of their relative explanatory pow evidence have been in short supply. This article employs national probabil conducted in 1994 to assess differences between Czechs and Slovaks in democratic norms and liberal attitudes with respect to economic, politi issues. The applicability of an explanation focusing on long-standing cultura the two countries is compared with a rational choice explanation based on in their recent experiences of political and economic transition. It is shown the expression of support for marketization and democracy in the two countr relatively parsimoniously in rational choice terms. The explanatory con culture appears to relate only to a narrow range of attitudes and values. The debate between 'political cultural' and 'rationalist' exp Eckstein among others has argued, is central to contemporary and to an understanding of political phenomena.l However fundamental and competing perspectives are not easy to f literature.2 The aim of this article is to take up the emp evaluating the value of political cultural versus rationalis context which arguably affords a good test of the exten usefulness - the case of the Czech Republic and Slovakia which theory offers the most appropriate and efficient expl on citizens' orientations towards liberalization. The term 'political culture' is famously (or notoriously) problematic even within the camp of scholars who find the term useful. There is little agreement among its practitioners on its scope and meaning, on how it may be measured, * Pembroke College, Oxford; and Nuffield College, Oxford, respectively. Responsibility for this article is held equally by the two authors. The research reported here was commissioned as part of the Economic and Social Research Council's East-West Programme, phase II, Grant no. Y309253025 'Emerging Forms of Political Representation and Participation in Eastern Europe'. The authors would like to thank Karen Henderson, Kieran Williams, Albert Weale and the Journal's three reviewers for comments on earlier drafts of this article. Harry Eckstein, 'A Culturalist Theory of Political Change', American Political Science Review, 82 (1988), 789-804. 2 An exception is Mitchell A. Seligson and John A. Booth, 'Political Culture and Regime Type: Evidence from Nicaragua and Costa Rica', Journal of Politics, 55 (1993), 777-92. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 130 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS130 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS or on its impact on political behaviour or political development.3 In th we do not propose a new definition or attempt to resolve the disputes various adherents of different conceptions of political culture ex rather, we seek to investigate the applicability of the 'subjectivist political culture - arguably the most influential one in recent res subjective approach to political culture theory was developed most Almond and Verba and more recently by Harry Eckstein.4 From this to explain an individual's actions or political responses in terms of cult assume that his/her preferences derive from normative orientatio mainly, but not exclusively, in childhood from the family and other of socialization and which are subject to relative continuity over time. is placed on shared values, beliefs and preferences in the political have been shaped by common historical understandings. The term culture' in this article, therefore, is taken to refer, as Archie Brown to 'the subjective perception of history and politics, the fundamental values, the foci of identification and loyalty, and the political kno expectations which are the product of the specific historical exp nations and groups'.5 An important characteristic of a political culture, at least in this su form, is that it is shared by large categories of people. William Re example, has argued that 'to the extent that the object of study is som culture, it must refer to an entire social grouping'.6 Or, as Elkins and put it, 'political culture is the property of a collectivity - nation, regi ethnic community' .7 Normally, the unit of political culture is the nati national political culture, in turn, is held in some contexts to constrain the choices taken in mass level politics and, thus, to account for national differences in certain political phenomena. Where differences are observable between nations with respect to a dimension of attitudes, such as willingness to tolerate non-conformist life-styles or to allow freedom of political protest, they would be explained by a culturalist in terms of long-standing societal norms transmitted through socialization especially during individuals' formative 3 For a recent critique, see William M. Reisinger, 'The Renaissance of a Rubric: Political Culture as Concept and Theory', International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 7 (1995), 328-52. See also John Street, 'Political Culture - From Civic Culture to Mass Culture', British Journal of Political Science, 24 (1994), 95-104; Archie Brown, 'Political Culture', in A. Kuper and J. Kuper, eds, The Social Science Encyclopaedia, 2nd edn (London and New York: Routledge, 1996); and Stephen Welch, The Concept of Political Culture (London: Macmillan, 1993). 4 Gabriel A. Almond and Sidney Verba, eds, The Civic Culture: Political Attitudes and Democracy in Five Nations (Princeton, NJ Princeton University Press, 1963); Eckstein, 'A Culturalist Theory of Political Change'. 5 Archie Brown, 'Introduction' in A. H. Brown and J. Gray, eds, Political Culture and Political Change in Communist States (London: Macmillan, 1977), p. 1. 6 Reisinger, 'The Renaissance of a Rubric: Political Culture as Concept and Theory', p. 337 (emphasis added). 7 David J. Elkins and Richard E. B. Simeon, 'A Cause in Search of Its Effect, or What Does Political Culture Explain?' Comparative Politics, 11 (1979), 127-45, p. 129. or on its impact on political behaviour or political development.3 In this article we do not propose a new definition or attempt to resolve the disputes among the various adherents of different conceptions of political culture explanations; rather, we seek to investigate the applicability of the 'subjectivist' variant of political culture - arguably the most influential one in recent research. The subjective approach to political culture theory was developed most notably by Almond and Verba and more recently by Harry Eckstein.4 From this perspective, to explain an individual's actions or political responses in terms of culture is to assume that his/her preferences derive from normative orientations learned mainly, but not exclusively, in childhood from the family and other institutions of socialization and which are subject to relative continuity over time. Emphasis is placed on shared values, beliefs and preferences in the political sphere that have been shaped by common historical understandings. The term 'political culture' in this article, therefore, is taken to refer, as Archie Brown has put it, to 'the subjective perception of history and politics, the fundamental beliefs and values, the foci of identification and loyalty, and the political knowledge and expectations which are the product of the specific historical experience of nations and groups'.5 An important characteristic of a political culture, at least in this subjectivist form, is that it is shared by large categories of people. William Reisinger, for example, has argued that 'to the extent that the object of study is some form of culture, it must refer to an entire social grouping'.6 Or, as Elkins and Simeon put it, 'political culture is the property of a collectivity - nation, region, class, ethnic community' .7 Normally, the unit of political culture is the nation, and this national political culture, in turn, is held in some contexts to constrain the choices taken in mass level politics and, thus, to account for national differences in certain political phenomena. Where differences are observable between nations with respect to a dimension of attitudes, such as willingness to tolerate non-conformist life-styles or to allow freedom of political protest, they would be explained by a culturalist in terms of long-standing societal norms transmitted through socialization especially during individuals' formative 3 For a recent critique, see William M. Reisinger, 'The Renaissance of a Rubric: Political Culture as Concept and Theory', International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 7 (1995), 328-52. See also John Street, 'Political Culture - From Civic Culture to Mass Culture', British Journal of Political Science, 24 (1994), 95-104; Archie Brown, 'Political Culture', in A. Kuper and J. Kuper, eds, The Social Science Encyclopaedia, 2nd edn (London and New York: Routledge, 1996); and Stephen Welch, The Concept of Political Culture (London: Macmillan, 1993). 4 Gabriel A. Almond and Sidney Verba, eds, The Civic Culture: Political Attitudes and Democracy in Five Nations (Princeton, NJ Princeton University Press, 1963); Eckstein, 'A Culturalist Theory of Political Change'. 5 Archie Brown, 'Introduction' in A. H. Brown and J. Gray, eds, Political Culture and Political Change in Communist States (London: Macmillan, 1977), p. 1. 6 Reisinger, 'The Renaissance of a Rubric: Political Culture as Concept and Theory', p. 337 (emphasis added). 7 David J. Elkins and Richard E. B. Simeon, 'A Cause in Search of Its Effect, or What Does Political Culture Explain?' Comparative Politics, 11 (1979), 127-45, p. 129. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice years. Thus, a political culture explanation posits a long and complex chain of causal explanation, involving historic experiences and foci of groups, which are transmitted to individuals in a shared manner especially via childhood and official socialization leading to a particular response to a current set of political circumstances. This form of explanation can usefully be counterposed with a rational choic one which emphasizes situational characteristics, including the social character istics of the agent, political opportunities and recent experience as fact shaping an individual's attitudes and behaviour. In this view, individuals construct and reconstruct their political responses and behaviour on the b of the combination of available information, resources and constraints. Such a approach is labelled 'rationalist' by Eckstein although, again, the extent a meaning of rationality is highly contested.8 Nevertheless, rather than expecti shared political values among national cultures resulting from the normat convergence of citizens via the socialization process on common nationa symbols, myths and foci of loyalty and identification, emphasis on the ration choice account is given to the importance of contemporary state contex individual endowments and opportunities for political voice. Thus, as in t example given above, where differences among nations on a dimension political attitudes are observed, this account would focus on the immedia political context and the recent political, economic and social experiences citizens. The aim of such an approach is to account for differences in national responses by reference to such proximal factors rather than historicall entrenched cultural differences. By comparison with the political cultu approach, therefore, rational choice explanation is rather direct and immediat in terms of the causal chain of processes required to produce a given attitudin response;9 individuals assess a given political issue in terms of their rec experience and calculated future opportunities The reader might at this point object to our characterization of the subjectivi variant of political culture theory. Certainly, in recent work Almond has argu against the sort of dichotomy we have just presented and in favour of approach to political culture that takes account of institutional factors as well 8 Kirsten Renwick Monroe, 'The Theory of Rational Action', in K. R. Monroe, ed., The Econom Approach to Politics (New York: Harper Collins, 1991). 9 Critics of rational choice approaches may argue that the apparent relative simplicity of t causal relationship between experience or opportunity and attitudes disappears when the high c of the subject's making the calculation are taken into account. By comparison, oriented action is to reduce transaction costs significantly and thus simplify the decision choice for agents. This v may be true if agents are being asked to optimize, though this is not an assumption of the notion o rational choice developed here. However, the supposed simplicity of oriented action from the po of view of transaction costs is not entirely clear; agents are still required to perform the complex t of deciding by some criterion under which of a number of possible orientations a particular cur or prospective phenomenon should be subsumed. It is not at all clear that such a mental enterpr is less costly than calculations based on referral to recent experience. years. Thus, a political culture explanation posits a long and complex chain causal explanation, involving historic experiences and foci of groups, which ar transmitted to individuals in a shared manner especially via childhood a official socialization leading to a particular response to a current set of politic circumstances. This form of explanation can usefully be counterposed with a rational choic one which emphasizes situational characteristics, including the social character istics of the agent, political opportunities and recent experience as fact shaping an individual's attitudes and behaviour. In this view, individuals construct and reconstruct their political responses and behaviour on the b of the combination of available information, resources and constraints. Such a approach is labelled 'rationalist' by Eckstein although, again, the extent a meaning of rationality is highly contested.8 Nevertheless, rather than expecti shared political values among national cultures resulting from the normat convergence of citizens via the socialization process on common nationa symbols, myths and foci of loyalty and identification, emphasis on the ration choice account is given to the importance of contemporary state contex individual endowments and opportunities for political voice. Thus, as in t example given above, where differences among nations on a dimension political attitudes are observed, this account would focus on the immedia political context and the recent political, economic and social experiences citizens. The aim of such an approach is to account for differences in national responses by reference to such proximal factors rather than historicall entrenched cultural differences. By comparison with the political cultu approach, therefore, rational choice explanation is rather direct and immediat in terms of the causal chain of processes required to produce a given attitudin response;9 individuals assess a given political issue in terms of their rec experience and calculated future opportunities The reader might at this point object to our characterization of the subjectivi variant of political culture theory. Certainly, in recent work Almond has argu against the sort of dichotomy we have just presented and in favour of approach to political culture that takes account of institutional factors as well 8 Kirsten Renwick Monroe, 'The Theory of Rational Action', in K. R. Monroe, ed., The Econom Approach to Politics (New York: Harper Collins, 1991). 9 Critics of rational choice approaches may argue that the apparent relative simplicity of t causal relationship between experience or opportunity and attitudes disappears when the high c of the subject's making the calculation are taken into account. By comparison, oriented action is to reduce transaction costs significantly and thus simplify the decision choice for agents. This v may be true if agents are being asked to optimize, though this is not an assumption of the notion o rational choice developed here. However, the supposed simplicity of oriented action from the po of view of transaction costs is not entirely clear; agents are still required to perform the complex t of deciding by some criterion under which of a number of possible orientations a particular cur or prospective phenomenon should be subsumed. It is not at all clear that such a mental enterpr is less costly than calculations based on referral to recent experience. 131131 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 132 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS132 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS as recent experience. 1 As he puts it, even 'the early advocates of p explanation recognised that causality worked both ways: attitu structure and behaviour, and structure and performance in t attitudes. There was essentially a straw man polemic.' Almond to cite numerous studies12 undertaken since his pioneering work w Verba which show that political culture has been subject to c considerable 'plasticity'.13 And he offers a definition of political c includes not only the factors present in our characterization above b such as 'adult experience with governmental, social and econom ance', concluding that the 'causal arrows between culture and s performance go both ways'.14 Our view is that under such a definition, the term 'politica essentially meaningless. This is not to say that reciprocal causa are unknown to science - though Almond makes no effort to conditions of reciprocal influence in advance of research, as approach would dictate. More importantly, if political cultur subjective socialized orientations and adult experience and eva performance, the term has become so broad that it is of little the There is the suspicion that in face of evidence of the failure o perfectly scientific - specification of the term, defenders of politic as Almond have so diluted the concept as to make it impossible or to refute. Rather than attacking a 'straw man' in our characteri subjectivist variant of political culture, therefore, we would argue seeking to test a theoretical construct that may turn out to be we against another approach which is, in advance of the test, equally possibility of failure; after all, if political culture theory is right, evident that recent experience is very likely to explain away diffe groups with distinctive long-standing historical foci of identification. At the very least, even on Almond's revised definit expect there to be a large residual of variance between societ unexplained by recent experience - if there were not, then th revised definition is even harder to see. 10 Gabriel A. Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', in Dirk Berg-Schlosser and Ralf Rytlewski, eds, Political Culture in Germany (London: Macmillan, 1993). 11 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture' p. 16. 12 For example: S. M. Lipset, and W. Schneider, The Confidence Gap (New York: Free Press/Macmillan, 1983); D. Kavanagh, 'Political Culture in Britain: The Decline of the Civic Culture', in G. Almond and S. Verba, eds, The Civic Culture Revisited (Boston, Mass.: Little, Brown, 1980); K. Baker, R. Dalton and K. Hildebrandt, Germany Transformed: Political Culture and the New Politics (Cambridge, Mass., and London: Harvard University Press, 1981); S. H. Barnes and M. Kaase et al., Political Action: Mass Participation in Five Western Democracies (Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage, 1979); and R. Inglehart, The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles Among Western Publics (Princeton, N. J: Princeton University Press, 1977). 13 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 18. 14 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 15. as recent experience. 1 As he puts it, even 'the early advocates of political culture explanation recognised that causality worked both ways: attitudes influence structure and behaviour, and structure and performance in turn influence attitudes. There was essentially a straw man polemic.' Almond then goes on to cite numerous studies12 undertaken since his pioneering work with Sydney Verba which show that political culture has been subject to change and considerable 'plasticity'.13 And he offers a definition of political culture which includes not only the factors present in our characterization above but also others such as 'adult experience with governmental, social and economic performance', concluding that the 'causal arrows between culture and structure and performance go both ways'.14 Our view is that under such a definition, the term 'political culture' is essentially meaningless. This is not to say that reciprocal causal relationships are unknown to science - though Almond makes no effort to stipulate the conditions of reciprocal influence in advance of research, as a scientific approach would dictate. More importantly, if political culture means both subjective socialized orientations and adult experience and evaluations of performance, the term has become so broad that it is of little theoretical value. There is the suspicion that in face of evidence of the failure of the initial perfectly scientific - specification of the term, defenders of political culture such as Almond have so diluted the concept as to make it impossible either to test or to refute. Rather than attacking a 'straw man' in our characterization of the subjectivist variant of political culture, therefore, we would argue that we are seeking to test a theoretical construct that may turn out to be weak or wrong against another approach which is, in advance of the test, equally subject to the possibility of failure; after all, if political culture theory is right, it is not at all evident that recent experience is very likely to explain away differences between groups with distinctive long-standing historical foci of loyalty and identification. At the very least, even on Almond's revised definition, we would expect there to be a large residual of variance between societies to remain unexplained by recent experience - if there were not, then the value of the revised definition is even harder to see. 10 Gabriel A. Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', in Dirk Berg-Schlosser and Ralf Rytlewski, eds, Political Culture in Germany (London: Macmillan, 1993). 11 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture' p. 16. 12 For example: S. M. Lipset, and W. Schneider, The Confidence Gap (New York: Free Press/Macmillan, 1983); D. Kavanagh, 'Political Culture in Britain: The Decline of the Civic Culture', in G. Almond and S. Verba, eds, The Civic Culture Revisited (Boston, Mass.: Little, Brown, 1980); K. Baker, R. Dalton and K. Hildebrandt, Germany Transformed: Political Culture and the New Politics (Cambridge, Mass., and London: Harvard University Press, 1981); S. H. Barnes and M. Kaase et al., Political Action: Mass Participation in Five Western Democracies (Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage, 1979); and R. Inglehart, The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles Among Western Publics (Princeton, N. J: Princeton University Press, 1977). 13 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 18. 14 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 15. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice In this sense, our explanatory strategy privileges political culture explanation; whatever is not accounted for by experience may be interpreted as evidence for the value of political culture. However, it is worth noting that the differences in the character of the two frameworks ought to incline us to prefer a rational choice account even if the two theories were equally valid in face of the evidence. If we were to apply a principle common to adjudicating between competing theories, Occam's razor, whereby simpler explanations of phenomena are preferred over more complex ones on grounds of parsimony and a desire to avoid multiplication of unnecessary causal factors, especially when the simpler ones are efficient, the advantages of the rational choice perspective are clear; as pointed out above, it requires much less complexity with respect to the causal relationship between the subject and the political object. As John Street has argued, to justify itself empirically political culture theory would have to demonstrate that it is an indispensable and decisive factor in an explanation over and above rational considerations.15 Thus, where experience and assessments of future prospects are able to account for differences between groups, we feel justified in asserting that the rational choice perspective is preferable to a political culture one. Fortunately, for a number of reasons, a case study of the Czech Republic and Slovakia is particularly appropriate to the evaluation of the relative merits of the two approaches. Indeed, many modem political culture studies were stimulated by questions arising from the impact of communism on societies such as Czechoslovakia.'6 The Czechoslovak case allows for clear hypotheses to be framed concerning cultural differences and their sources which enable the complexities of historically transmitted culture versus rational choice responses to be disentangled. First, the two countries had distinctive political histories and national traditions before 1918, and some commentators have argued that these traditions reasserted themselves since 1989. As a result of their distinctive histories, important divergences in political orientations are evident among the peoples of the two nations. As Miroslav Kusy has observed, a stereotype of argument about Czechoslovakia was that 'Slovaks were too different from Czechs, and so their state was only an artificially and forcibly maintained entity'.7 Five areas of difference have been especially emphasized by a number of authors: Slovaks are more nationalistic, more separatist (on ethnic questions), more Christianconservative (on individual rights and tolerance), more left-wing (on the economy), and more 'eastward-looking'.18 15 Street, 'Political Culture - From Civic Culture to Mass Culture', pp. 96-7. 16 Brown and Gray, eds, Political Culture and Political Change in Communist States. 17 Miroslav Kusy, 'Slovak Exceptionalism', in J. Musil, ed., The End of Czechoslovakia (Budapest: Central European University Press, 1995), p. 139. 18 See Kusy, 'Slovak Exceptionalism'; John Morison, 'The Road to Separation: Nationalism in Czechoslovakia', in Paul Latawski, ed., Contemporary Nationalism in East Central Europe (London: Macmillan, 1995), pp. 67-86; and Archie Brown and Gordon Wightman, 'Czechoslovakia: Revival In this sense, our explanatory strategy privileges political culture explanation; whatever is not accounted for by experience may be interpreted as evidence for the value of political culture. However, it is worth noting that the differences in the character of the two frameworks ought to incline us to prefer a rational choice account even if the two theories were equally valid in face of the evidence. If we were to apply a principle common to adjudicating between competing theories, Occam's razor, whereby simpler explanations of phenomena are preferred over more complex ones on grounds of parsimony and a desire to avoid multiplication of unnecessary causal factors, especially when the simpler ones are efficient, the advantages of the rational choice perspective are clear; as pointed out above, it requires much less complexity with respect to the causal relationship between the subject and the political object. As John Street has argued, to justify itself empirically political culture theory would have to demonstrate that it is an indispensable and decisive factor in an explanation over and above rational considerations.15 Thus, where experience and assessments of future prospects are able to account for differences between groups, we feel justified in asserting that the rational choice perspective is preferable to a political culture one. Fortunately, for a number of reasons, a case study of the Czech Republic and Slovakia is particularly appropriate to the evaluation of the relative merits of the two approaches. Indeed, many modem political culture studies were stimulated by questions arising from the impact of communism on societies such as Czechoslovakia.'6 The Czechoslovak case allows for clear hypotheses to be framed concerning cultural differences and their sources which enable the complexities of historically transmitted culture versus rational choice responses to be disentangled. First, the two countries had distinctive political histories and national traditions before 1918, and some commentators have argued that these traditions reasserted themselves since 1989. As a result of their distinctive histories, important divergences in political orientations are evident among the peoples of the two nations. As Miroslav Kusy has observed, a stereotype of argument about Czechoslovakia was that 'Slovaks were too different from Czechs, and so their state was only an artificially and forcibly maintained entity'.7 Five areas of difference have been especially emphasized by a number of authors: Slovaks are more nationalistic, more separatist (on ethnic questions), more Christianconservative (on individual rights and tolerance), more left-wing (on the economy), and more 'eastward-looking'.18 15 Street, 'Political Culture - From Civic Culture to Mass Culture', pp. 96-7. 16 Brown and Gray, eds, Political Culture and Political Change in Communist States. 17 Miroslav Kusy, 'Slovak Exceptionalism', in J. Musil, ed., The End of Czechoslovakia (Budapest: Central European University Press, 1995), p. 139. 18 See Kusy, 'Slovak Exceptionalism'; John Morison, 'The Road to Separation: Nationalism in Czechoslovakia', in Paul Latawski, ed., Contemporary Nationalism in East Central Europe (London: Macmillan, 1995), pp. 67-86; and Archie Brown and Gordon Wightman, 'Czechoslovakia: Revival 133133 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 134 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS134 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS Secondly, and in clear contrast to this view of historically-based ness, the creation of a unified Czechoslovakia in 1918 may h departure from cultural diversity. Twenty years of inter-war dem and political unity were followed by more than forty years attempts by the communist authorities to introduce new soci similar patterns of social structure across the Czech and Slovak la there is considerable evidence of success in this latter regard, respect to equalization in many aspects of economic, ed demographic indicators.19 The salient political culture in the p period, therefore, might be that related to the historical ex transformations brought about during the political unity of t rather than the residue of historical events before that time. Accor of distinctive political orientations, a relative similarity in the po of Czechs and Slovaks might have emerged. Thirdly, however, contemporary evidence for similarity or dive structure of attitudes among Czechs and Slovaks need not support political culture explanations. Diversity need not be the result of political traditions - nor need similarity be accounted for by communist socialization. The alternative, rational choice, acco to explain differences in terms of variation in the recent experie and environment of the two peoples. One expectation of a suc culture explanation would be that it should demonstrate t differences referred to by Reisinger and Elkins and Simeon are e or notwithstanding differences among group members in ter situation or experience. Where group differences can be accounted of current situation or experience, however, evidence for the political culture would be absent and the rational choice explan supported. The strategy of the rest of the article is to assess and comp Slovak political cultures in the light of these competing appr differences in 'political culture' - the responses of Czechs an variety of important dimensions of political issues - are obser will be made to account for them, where possible, by reference t (F'note continued) or Retreat', in Brown and Gray, eds., Political Culture and Political Change in C pp. 159-96. These authors recognize that shared political history since 1918, with has resulted in convergence between the two cultures on some dimensions; none th evidence which suggests important differences in the historical identities of th have important political implications for their future orientations to democra remembered that the Czechs too have had an ambiguous attitude towards the W Germany. 19 Milan Kucera and Zdenik Pavlik, 'Czech and Slovak Demography'; Vaclav Prucha, 'Economic Developments and Relations, 1918-89'; and Jiri Musil, 'Czech and Slovak Society', all in Musil, ed., The End of Czechoslovakia, pp. 15-39, 40-76, and 77-96, respectively. Secondly, and in clear contrast to this view of historically-based distinctiveness, the creation of a unified Czechoslovakia in 1918 may have signalled a departure from cultural diversity. Twenty years of inter-war democratic consent and political unity were followed by more than forty years of aggressive attempts by the communist authorities to introduce new societal norms and similar patterns of social structure across the Czech and Slovak lands. Moreover, there is considerable evidence of success in this latter regard, especially with respect to equalization in many aspects of economic, educational and demographic indicators.19 The salient political culture in the post-communist period, therefore, might be that related to the historical experience and transformations brought about during the political unity of the two peoples, rather than the residue of historical events before that time. Accordingly, instead of distinctive political orientations, a relative similarity in the political cultures of Czechs and Slovaks might have emerged. Thirdly, however, contemporary evidence for similarity or diversity in the structure of attitudes among Czechs and Slovaks need not support either of these political culture explanations. Diversity need not be the result of continuity in political traditions - nor need similarity be accounted for by the success of communist socialization. The alternative, rational choice, account would seek to explain differences in terms of variation in the recent experiences, prospects and environment of the two peoples. One expectation of a successful political culture explanation would be that it should demonstrate that the group differences referred to by Reisinger and Elkins and Simeon are evident, despite or notwithstanding differences among group members in terms of current situation or experience. Where group differences can be accounted for in terms of current situation or experience, however, evidence for the importance of political culture would be absent and the rational choice explanation would be supported. The strategy of the rest of the article is to assess and compare Czech and Slovak political cultures in the light of these competing approaches. Where differences in 'political culture' - the responses of Czechs and Slovaks to a variety of important dimensions of political issues - are observed, an attempt will be made to account for them, where possible, by reference to differences (F'note continued) or Retreat', in Brown and Gray, eds., Political Culture and Political Change in Communist States, pp. 159-96. These authors recognize that shared political history since 1918, with a brief interlude, has resulted in convergence between the two cultures on some dimensions; none the less, they provide evidence which suggests important differences in the historical identities of the two peoples which have important political implications for their future orientations to democracy. It must also be remembered that the Czechs too have had an ambiguous attitude towards the West, particularly to Germany. 19 Milan Kucera and Zdenik Pavlik, 'Czech and Slovak Demography'; Vaclav Prucha, 'Economic Developments and Relations, 1918-89'; and Jiri Musil, 'Czech and Slovak Society', all in Musil, ed., The End of Czechoslovakia, pp. 15-39, 40-76, and 77-96, respectively. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice in the recent experiences of individuals in the two countries. If it is possible to explain national variation in this way, we would feel justified in, if not rejecting culturalist explanations outright, then at least in claiming, by Occam's razor, that they are not necessary to explain observed national differences in a context in which these sorts of theories have traditionally been prevalent and that, therefore, the concept of political culture is scientifically superfluous. The article is structured as follows: Section I uses batteries of questions in national surveys to operationalize certain key dimensions of subjective political culture and provides evidence on differences between Czechs and Slovaks on these dimensions. Important differences do emerge and these are generally with one important exception - consistent with the expectations of those, generally political culture, theorists, who have commented on differences between Czechs and Slovaks. Section II considers evidence on recent economic and political experiences of individuals in each country. Measures of these experiences are then used as independent variables in a number of regression models which attempt to explain statistically national differences on the dimensions of political culture presented in Section I. The results of these analyses tend to support rational choice explanations, in that differences in democratic and market commitment are explained by experience - although the admittedly somewhat less pronounced disparities with respect to social and political liberalism do seem to result from long-standing differences in the countries' religious traditions and thus offer support to a political culture explanation in at least this area. Section III addresses the question of the notable exception to expected differences in attitudes: contrary to expectations (and contemporary political practice): Slovaks do not appear to be less liberal than Czechs with respect to ethnic rights. The explanation for these findings, however - explored in Section III - appears to lie in the different meanings assigned to the questions in each country which result from the very different ethnic and state-building contexts faced by the two peoples. I. ONE POLITICAL CULTURE OR TWO (OR THREE)? Judging only on the basis of the nature of issues most salient to elite po and from the distinctive nature of the ruling parties in the two states sin there is a strong primafacie case for the existence of distinctive political c in the Czech Republic and Slovakia. These divergences, at least in eli competition, have become even more evident in the period since sepa Consistent with many expectations of differences in political culture, th 20 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'The Parting of the Ways? Explaining I Cleavage Formation in the Czech and Slovak Republics', Political Studies, 46 (1998 Sharon Fisher, 'Slovakia: Turning Back', Transition, 1994 in Review, Part I (Prague: O Research Institute, 1995); Sharon Fisher, 'Prime Minister and President Grapple for Transition, 11 (1995), 38-3; and Jiri Pehe, 'Czech Republic: A Leader in Political Stab Economic Growth', Transition, 1994 in Review, Part I (Prague: Open Media Research 1995). in the recent experiences of individuals in the two countries. If it is possible to explain national variation in this way, we would feel justified in, if not rejecting culturalist explanations outright, then at least in claiming, by Occam's razor, that they are not necessary to explain observed national differences in a context in which these sorts of theories have traditionally been prevalent and that, therefore, the concept of political culture is scientifically superfluous. The article is structured as follows: Section I uses batteries of questions in national surveys to operationalize certain key dimensions of subjective political culture and provides evidence on differences between Czechs and Slovaks on these dimensions. Important differences do emerge and these are generally with one important exception - consistent with the expectations of those, generally political culture, theorists, who have commented on differences between Czechs and Slovaks. Section II considers evidence on recent economic and political experiences of individuals in each country. Measures of these experiences are then used as independent variables in a number of regression models which attempt to explain statistically national differences on the dimensions of political culture presented in Section I. The results of these analyses tend to support rational choice explanations, in that differences in democratic and market commitment are explained by experience - although the admittedly somewhat less pronounced disparities with respect to social and political liberalism do seem to result from long-standing differences in the countries' religious traditions and thus offer support to a political culture explanation in at least this area. Section III addresses the question of the notable exception to expected differences in attitudes: contrary to expectations (and contemporary political practice): Slovaks do not appear to be less liberal than Czechs with respect to ethnic rights. The explanation for these findings, however - explored in Section III - appears to lie in the different meanings assigned to the questions in each country which result from the very different ethnic and state-building contexts faced by the two peoples. I. ONE POLITICAL CULTURE OR TWO (OR THREE)? Judging only on the basis of the nature of issues most salient to elite po and from the distinctive nature of the ruling parties in the two states sin there is a strong primafacie case for the existence of distinctive political c in the Czech Republic and Slovakia. These divergences, at least in eli competition, have become even more evident in the period since sepa Consistent with many expectations of differences in political culture, th 20 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'The Parting of the Ways? Explaining I Cleavage Formation in the Czech and Slovak Republics', Political Studies, 46 (1998 Sharon Fisher, 'Slovakia: Turning Back', Transition, 1994 in Review, Part I (Prague: O Research Institute, 1995); Sharon Fisher, 'Prime Minister and President Grapple for Transition, 11 (1995), 38-3; and Jiri Pehe, 'Czech Republic: A Leader in Political Stab Economic Growth', Transition, 1994 in Review, Part I (Prague: Open Media Research 1995). 135135 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 136 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS136 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS successful parties in the Czech Republic have chosen to compet economic questions and the ruling Civic Democratic Party has support and broad popularity by standing with a pro-market pro-European stance. Although other parties have appealed to voter points on each of these dimensions - the Communist Party on the with greater misgivings about the West, the Christian an Union/Czechoslovak People's party on more traditional Catholi and the Republican Party on nationalist and ethnic issues, anti-German sentiments21 - elite level conflict over the terms and of democracy itself has been notably absent. In Slovakia, by contrast, parties have presented a much more com issues to the electorate. The national question was first put on Slovak parties such as the Movement for a Democratic Slovakia extreme Slovak National Party. Moreover, the latter party also def antagonism to the interests of neighbouring Hungary and to t residing in Slovakia who constitute slightly more than 10 pe population.22 This, in turn, cemented the position of the vari Hungarian parties which operated electorally under a coalition.2 however, did not push economic or social questions wholly ou discussion: the Movement for a Democratic Slovakia was distinc its economic appeal, while the economic left was more co represented by the communist-successor Party of the Democratic the Christian Democratic Movement, though non-denominati an amalgam of economically right, nationalist and socially positions.24 From this brief description a picture emerges of the relative pr in the Czech Republic of support for the market, democratic norm political liberalism and ethnic rights when compared with Slovakia extent is such a picture from the elite level justified at the ma on citizens' beliefs? We address this issue by using data drawn from a questionnaire designed by the authors and administered to national probability 21 Gordon Wightman, 'The Czech and Slovak Republics', in Stephen White, Judy Batt and Paul Lewis, eds, Developments in East European Politics (London: Macmillan, 1993), pp. 51-65; Jan Obrman, 'The Czechoslovak Elections: A Guide to the Parties', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 22 (1992), 1-16; David Olson, 'Dissolution of the State: Political Parties and the 1992 Election in Czechoslovakia', Communists and Post-Communist Studies, 26 (1993), 301-14. 22 Statisticka rocenka Ceske a Slovenske Federativni Republiky 1990 (Prague: 1990), p. 100, cited in Karen Henderson, 'Czechoslovakia: The Failure of Consensus Politics' (University of Leicester Discussion Paper in Politics, 1993). The largest ethnic minority in the Czech Republic are Slovaks, who have generally been well assimilated; see Jiri Pehe, 'Slovaks in the Czech Republic: A New Minority', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 23 (1993), 59-62. 23 Alfred A. Reisch, 'Hungarian Ethnic Parties Prepare for Czechoslovak Elections', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 18 (1992), 26-32. 24 Zora Butorova and Martin Butora, 'Political Parties, Value Orientations and Slovakia's Road to Independence', in Gordon Wightman, ed., Party Formation in East-Central Europe (London: Edward Elgar, 1995), pp. 107-33. successful parties in the Czech Republic have chosen to compete over mainly economic questions and the ruling Civic Democratic Party has won electoral support and broad popularity by standing with a pro-market secular and pro-European stance. Although other parties have appealed to voters at different points on each of these dimensions - the Communist Party on the economic left with greater misgivings about the West, the Christian and Democratic Union/Czechoslovak People's party on more traditional Catholic social issues, and the Republican Party on nationalist and ethnic issues, especially anti-German sentiments21 - elite level conflict over the terms and institutions of democracy itself has been notably absent. In Slovakia, by contrast, parties have presented a much more complex set of issues to the electorate. The national question was first put on the agenda by Slovak parties such as the Movement for a Democratic Slovakia and the more extreme Slovak National Party. Moreover, the latter party also defined itself in antagonism to the interests of neighbouring Hungary and to the Hungarians residing in Slovakia who constitute slightly more than 10 per cent of the population.22 This, in turn, cemented the position of the various ethnically Hungarian parties which operated electorally under a coalition.23 These issues, however, did not push economic or social questions wholly out of political discussion: the Movement for a Democratic Slovakia was distinctly populist in its economic appeal, while the economic left was more conventionally represented by the communist-successor Party of the Democratic Left. Finally, the Christian Democratic Movement, though non-denominational, stood on an amalgam of economically right, nationalist and socially conservative positions.24 From this brief description a picture emerges of the relative preponderance in the Czech Republic of support for the market, democratic norms, social and political liberalism and ethnic rights when compared with Slovakia. But to what extent is such a picture from the elite level justified at the mass level by data on citizens' beliefs? We address this issue by using data drawn from a questionnaire designed by the authors and administered to national probability 21 Gordon Wightman, 'The Czech and Slovak Republics', in Stephen White, Judy Batt and Paul Lewis, eds, Developments in East European Politics (London: Macmillan, 1993), pp. 51-65; Jan Obrman, 'The Czechoslovak Elections: A Guide to the Parties', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 22 (1992), 1-16; David Olson, 'Dissolution of the State: Political Parties and the 1992 Election in Czechoslovakia', Communists and Post-Communist Studies, 26 (1993), 301-14. 22 Statisticka rocenka Ceske a Slovenske Federativni Republiky 1990 (Prague: 1990), p. 100, cited in Karen Henderson, 'Czechoslovakia: The Failure of Consensus Politics' (University of Leicester Discussion Paper in Politics, 1993). The largest ethnic minority in the Czech Republic are Slovaks, who have generally been well assimilated; see Jiri Pehe, 'Slovaks in the Czech Republic: A New Minority', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 23 (1993), 59-62. 23 Alfred A. Reisch, 'Hungarian Ethnic Parties Prepare for Czechoslovak Elections', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 18 (1992), 26-32. 24 Zora Butorova and Martin Butora, 'Political Parties, Value Orientations and Slovakia's Road to Independence', in Gordon Wightman, ed., Party Formation in East-Central Europe (London: Edward Elgar, 1995), pp. 107-33. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice samples of the adult populations of both states in the spring of 1994.25 Each survey contains batteries of questions indexing the core dimensions of political culture where, on the basis of expectations of mass level differences and the elite ideologies described above, citizens of the two states are expected to differ: on the extent of democratic commitment, market support, social liberalism and tolerance for diversity of opinion and life-style, and willingness to support the rights of ethnic minorities. All of these attitudes are measured using statements with five-point agree/disagree response formats, or variants thereof. In a few examples (see Table 2 below) respondents are presented with two contrasting policies and asked to say with which they agree most. Question wording was identical in the two countries except, of course, where the name of the country was referred to explicitly. As well as comparing Czech and Slovak responses to these questions we also distinguish between ethnic Slovaks and ethnic Hungarians. In Slovakia the interests of the Hungarian minority have been pressed not only by Hungarian parties, but by the keen interest of the Hungarian government between 1990 and 1994, for whom the issue of protection of the rights of Hungarians abroad constituted one of the central political issues.26 The extent to which Hungarians and Slovaks differ from or resemble one another on the various dimensions of attitudes is thus of evident relevance to any assessment of a 'Slovak' political culture. Attitudes Towards Democracy The first dimension on which differences between Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians have been anticipated relates to the level of commitment to democracy among the three groups. Slovaks in particular, as a result of the relative weakness of their democratic traditions, would be expected by many commentators to exhibit weaker levels of support for democratic norms. 25 Respondents were sampled from the lists of voters in the 1992 elections (access to current lists was not allowed under Czech and Slovak law), with minor augmentation using random route procedures and a small top-up sample for urban areas where the rate of non-contact due to movement was especially high. The Czech sample involved selection of 182 sampling points from which 2,104 addresses were chosen (names issued: 2,104; non-contact: 404; refusal 291; plus quota: 111; final achieved sample: 1,520; the response rate was 67 per cent of the initial sample and 83 per cent of those contacted). The Slovak sample involved selection of 215 sampling points from which 2,014 addresses were chosen; (names issued: 2,014; non-contact: 338; refusal 233; plus quota: 68; final achieved sample: 1,509; the response rate was 75 per cent of the initial sample and 86 per cent of those contacted). The surveys were directed by Lubos Rezler and Jan Hartl of the Stredisko empirickych vyzkumu (STEM), from its offices in Prague and Bratislava. 26 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'Social and Ideological Cleavage Formation in Post-Communist Hungary', Europe-Asia Studies, 47 (1995), 1177-204; Alfred A. Reisch, 'Hungarian-Slovak Relations: A Difficult First Year', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 50 (1993), 16-23. samples of the adult populations of both states in the spring of 1994.25 Each survey contains batteries of questions indexing the core dimensions of political culture where, on the basis of expectations of mass level differences and the elite ideologies described above, citizens of the two states are expected to differ: on the extent of democratic commitment, market support, social liberalism and tolerance for diversity of opinion and life-style, and willingness to support the rights of ethnic minorities. All of these attitudes are measured using statements with five-point agree/disagree response formats, or variants thereof. In a few examples (see Table 2 below) respondents are presented with two contrasting policies and asked to say with which they agree most. Question wording was identical in the two countries except, of course, where the name of the country was referred to explicitly. As well as comparing Czech and Slovak responses to these questions we also distinguish between ethnic Slovaks and ethnic Hungarians. In Slovakia the interests of the Hungarian minority have been pressed not only by Hungarian parties, but by the keen interest of the Hungarian government between 1990 and 1994, for whom the issue of protection of the rights of Hungarians abroad constituted one of the central political issues.26 The extent to which Hungarians and Slovaks differ from or resemble one another on the various dimensions of attitudes is thus of evident relevance to any assessment of a 'Slovak' political culture. Attitudes Towards Democracy The first dimension on which differences between Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians have been anticipated relates to the level of commitment to democracy among the three groups. Slovaks in particular, as a result of the relative weakness of their democratic traditions, would be expected by many commentators to exhibit weaker levels of support for democratic norms. 25 Respondents were sampled from the lists of voters in the 1992 elections (access to current lists was not allowed under Czech and Slovak law), with minor augmentation using random route procedures and a small top-up sample for urban areas where the rate of non-contact due to movement was especially high. The Czech sample involved selection of 182 sampling points from which 2,104 addresses were chosen (names issued: 2,104; non-contact: 404; refusal 291; plus quota: 111; final achieved sample: 1,520; the response rate was 67 per cent of the initial sample and 83 per cent of those contacted). The Slovak sample involved selection of 215 sampling points from which 2,014 addresses were chosen; (names issued: 2,014; non-contact: 338; refusal 233; plus quota: 68; final achieved sample: 1,509; the response rate was 75 per cent of the initial sample and 86 per cent of those contacted). The surveys were directed by Lubos Rezler and Jan Hartl of the Stredisko empirickych vyzkumu (STEM), from its offices in Prague and Bratislava. 26 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'Social and Ideological Cleavage Formation in Post-Communist Hungary', Europe-Asia Studies, 47 (1995), 1177-204; Alfred A. Reisch, 'Hungarian-Slovak Relations: A Difficult First Year', RFE/RL Research Report, No. 50 (1993), 16-23. 137137 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 138 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS138 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS TABLE 1 Attitudes Towards Democracy by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Do you agree with the aim of Agree 73% 59% 63% introducing democracy in the Disagree 26% 36% 33% country, in which political parties compete for power Democracy gives ordinary Agree 48% 42% 38% people more say in how the Disagree 29% 37% 40% country is run Democracy is a good means Agree 43% 39% 46% of solving social conflicts Disagree 21% 32% 28% Democracy is better for the Agree 45% 57% 62% rich in society than the poor Disagree 34% 25% 18% TABLE 1 Attitudes Towards Democracy by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Do you agree with the aim of Agree 73% 59% 63% introducing democracy in the Disagree 26% 36% 33% country, in which political parties compete for power Democracy gives ordinary Agree 48% 42% 38% people more say in how the Disagree 29% 37% 40% country is run Democracy is a good means Agree 43% 39% 46% of solving social conflicts Disagree 21% 32% 28% Democracy is better for the Agree 45% 57% 62% rich in society than the poor Disagree 34% 25% 18% Table 1 presents the distribution of responses to a battery of questions three groups.27 The evidence in Table 1 indicates that there are significant diff between the populations of the two states. These differences are cleare first item, in which respondents are asked to express the strength agreement with the aim of introducing democracy in their country. Thi thus taps into the normative foundations of democratic support: C clearly more supportive than Slovaks, with Hungarian responses lying the two main ethnic groups. Slovaks are also less willing to accept d as a good means of solving social conflicts than Czechs, although in Hungarians are not significantly different from Czechs. As we have di elsewhere in Eastern Europe where minorities face difficulties in integra the democratic process - in Estonia, for example - a sense of having in the democratic process has been associated with a greater desire democracy as a means of resolving social conflicts.28 On the remaining two items, however, ethnic Slovaks are again than Czechs to believe that democracy gives ordinary people more say; surprisingly given their minority status and the rhetoric of many lead politicians, Slovak Hungarians are even less likely to agree with this sta 27 The number of respondents to the questions presented in this table and those whic within the following ranges: Czechs: n = 1,456-1,461; Slovaks: n = 1,285-1,291; Slov ians: n = 172-173. 28 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'The Politics and Economics of Democra Commitment', British Journal of Political Science, 25 (1995), 485-514. Table 1 presents the distribution of responses to a battery of questions by the three groups.27 The evidence in Table 1 indicates that there are significant difference between the populations of the two states. These differences are clearest on th first item, in which respondents are asked to express the strength of the agreement with the aim of introducing democracy in their country. This questio thus taps into the normative foundations of democratic support: Czechs a clearly more supportive than Slovaks, with Hungarian responses lying between the two main ethnic groups. Slovaks are also less willing to accept democra as a good means of solving social conflicts than Czechs, although in this ca Hungarians are not significantly different from Czechs. As we have discovered elsewhere in Eastern Europe where minorities face difficulties in integrating int the democratic process - in Estonia, for example - a sense of having little s in the democratic process has been associated with a greater desire to endo democracy as a means of resolving social conflicts.28 On the remaining two items, however, ethnic Slovaks are again less like than Czechs to believe that democracy gives ordinary people more say; but, no surprisingly given their minority status and the rhetoric of many leading Slo politicians, Slovak Hungarians are even less likely to agree with this statement 27 The number of respondents to the questions presented in this table and those which follow within the following ranges: Czechs: n = 1,456-1,461; Slovaks: n = 1,285-1,291; Slovak Hung ians: n = 172-173. 28 Geoffrey Evans and Stephen Whitefield, 'The Politics and Economics of Democra Commitment', British Journal of Political Science, 25 (1995), 485-514. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice The responses of the groups to the question of whether democracy is better for the rich in society than the poor show a similar pattern: with Czechs again more committed to democracy than are either of the two Slovak groups. Attitudes Towards the Market Evidence on differences between the states in normative support for the m and redistributive intervention in the economy also offers support fo culturalist account. Czechs appear significantly more supportive of ma principles than either Slovaks or Slovak Hungarians. But, again, the fac Hungarians are similar to their Slovak compatriots on these items might al taken as testimony to the importance of countrywide economic circumstan in determining the level of market support. The variations which appeared in the relative levels of support for democ especially in the responses of Slovaks are, however, largely absent w considering the other items presented in Table 2 - relative levels of suppor the market are remarkably similar across a whole range of measures Czechs are clearly more likely than ethnic Slovaks - and in most case Hungarians - to agree that governments should not intervene to secur provision, income equality, state ownership and control over wages, prices profits. They are also much more likely to believe that private enterprise i best way to solve the country's problems, and that large differences in inc are important for the country's prosperity. This is not to say that Czechs regarded as markedly laissez-faire on the economy: for example, the overwhelming majority believe that governments should ensure guaranteed basic incomes, and a majority also believe that governments should ensure jobs for all and a decent standard of living. It is even the case that a majority of Czechs support maintaining major public services in state ownership. However, Czechs do emerge as significantly more supportive of the free market than Slovaks. Social Liberalism Some political culturalists have emphasized the importance of long-s differences between the two populations with respect to issues of liberalism: Slovaks with their 'Eastward-looking', more orthodox and outlook have been regarded as having a less tolerant set of orientat Certainly, the Slovaks in our sample were far more likely to have a r denomination (73 per cent were Catholics) and to attend church regu per cent reported attending once a week or more often) than were th (34 per cent and 5 per cent, respectively).29 Table 3 provides data on a range of questions which tap into the liberalism dimension of political culture, covering issues such as the e The responses of the groups to the question of whether democracy is bet the rich in society than the poor show a similar pattern: with Czechs aga committed to democracy than are either of the two Slovak groups. Attitudes Towards the Market Evidence on differences between the states in normative support for the m and redistributive intervention in the economy also offers support fo culturalist account. Czechs appear significantly more supportive of ma principles than either Slovaks or Slovak Hungarians. But, again, the fac Hungarians are similar to their Slovak compatriots on these items might al taken as testimony to the importance of countrywide economic circumstan in determining the level of market support. The variations which appeared in the relative levels of support for democ especially in the responses of Slovaks are, however, largely absent w considering the other items presented in Table 2 - relative levels of suppor the market are remarkably similar across a whole range of measures Czechs are clearly more likely than ethnic Slovaks - and in most case Hungarians - to agree that governments should not intervene to secur provision, income equality, state ownership and control over wages, prices profits. They are also much more likely to believe that private enterprise i best way to solve the country's problems, and that large differences in inc are important for the country's prosperity. This is not to say that Czechs regarded as markedly laissez-faire on the economy: for example, the overwhelming majority believe that governments should ensure guaranteed basic incomes, and a majority also believe that governments should ensure jobs for all and a decent standard of living. It is even the case that a majority of Czechs support maintaining major public services in state ownership. However, Czechs do emerge as significantly more supportive of the free market than Slovaks. Social Liberalism Some political culturalists have emphasized the importance of long-s differences between the two populations with respect to issues of liberalism: Slovaks with their 'Eastward-looking', more orthodox and outlook have been regarded as having a less tolerant set of orientat Certainly, the Slovaks in our sample were far more likely to have a r denomination (73 per cent were Catholics) and to attend church regu per cent reported attending once a week or more often) than were th (34 per cent and 5 per cent, respectively).29 Table 3 provides data on a range of questions which tap into the liberalism dimension of political culture, covering issues such as the e 29 Further information on the demographic characteristics of the samples can be obtai the authors. 29 Further information on the demographic characteristics of the samples can be obtained from the authors. 139139 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 140 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS140 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS TABLE 2 Attitudes Towards Markets and Government Involvem Ethnic Group TABLE 2 Attitudes Towards Markets and Government Involvem Ethnic Group Czechs SlovaksCzechs Slovaks The government should ensure that every person has a job and a good standard of living or The government should just let each person get ahead on their own The government should not concern itself with inequality or The government should try to minimize income differences The government should take all major industries into state ownership or The government should place all major industries in private ownership Individual companies should decide wages, prices and profits or The government should control wages, prices and profits Private enterprise is best Major public services ought to be in state ownership Large differences in income are necessary for prosperity Profits are the best way to improve everyone's standard of living Government should reduce the difference between high and low incomes Government should provide a guaranteed basic income The government should ensure that every person has a job and a good standard of living or The government should just let each person get ahead on their own The government should not concern itself with inequality or The government should try to minimize income differences The government should take all major industries into state ownership or The government should place all major industries in private ownership Individual companies should decide wages, prices and profits or The government should control wages, prices and profits Private enterprise is best Major public services ought to be in state ownership Large differences in income are necessary for prosperity Profits are the best way to improve everyone's standard of living Government should reduce the difference between high and low incomes Government should provide a guaranteed basic income Agree 66% 80%Agree 66% 80% Slovak Hungarians 78% Slovak Hungarians 78% Agree 11% 5% 9% Agree 29% 40% 41% Agree 45% 39% 38% Agree 47% 59% 49% Agree 21% 14% 15% Agree 29% 40% 41% Agree 49% 42% 39% Agree 50% 34% 28% Disagree 18% 30% 32% Agree 57% 62% 62% Disagree 23% 17% 14% Agree 24% 14% 20% Disagree 42% 57% 52% Agree 49% 42% 38% Disagree 25% 32% 28% Agree 37% 46% 56% Disagree 37% 30% 19% Agree 83% 88% 80% Disagree 9% 6% 6% Agree 11% 5% 9% Agree 29% 40% 41% Agree 45% 39% 38% Agree 47% 59% 49% Agree 21% 14% 15% Agree 29% 40% 41% Agree 49% 42% 39% Agree 50% 34% 28% Disagree 18% 30% 32% Agree 57% 62% 62% Disagree 23% 17% 14% Agree 24% 14% 20% Disagree 42% 57% 52% Agree 49% 42% 38% Disagree 25% 32% 28% Agree 37% 46% 56% Disagree 37% 30% 19% Agree 83% 88% 80% Disagree 9% 6% 6% This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice TABLE 3 Attitudes Towards Social Liberalism by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians People should be allowed to Agree 86% 90% 91% worship in their own way Disagree 5% 4% 2% Young people don't respect Agree 13% 16% 16% traditional values Disagree 64% 60% 58% Censorship of films and Agree 48% 43% 50% magazines is necessary to Disagree 30% 34% 32% uphold moral standards People should be allowed Agree 57% 49% 59% to organize public protest Disagree 17% 22% 15% against government Homosexual relationships Agree 22% 14% 16% are always wrong Disagree 44% 52% 45% People should be more tolerant of Agree 44% 43% 39% unconventional lifestyles Disagree 22% 21% 21% This country needs Agree 31% 27% 25% government with a strong hand Disagree 47% 52% 56% People should be Agree 72% 65% 64% free to emigrate even if Disagree 11% 18% 17% their skills are needed TABLE 3 Attitudes Towards Social Liberalism by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians People should be allowed to Agree 86% 90% 91% worship in their own way Disagree 5% 4% 2% Young people don't respect Agree 13% 16% 16% traditional values Disagree 64% 60% 58% Censorship of films and Agree 48% 43% 50% magazines is necessary to Disagree 30% 34% 32% uphold moral standards People should be allowed Agree 57% 49% 59% to organize public protest Disagree 17% 22% 15% against government Homosexual relationships Agree 22% 14% 16% are always wrong Disagree 44% 52% 45% People should be more tolerant of Agree 44% 43% 39% unconventional lifestyles Disagree 22% 21% 21% This country needs Agree 31% 27% 25% government with a strong hand Disagree 47% 52% 56% People should be Agree 72% 65% 64% free to emigrate even if Disagree 11% 18% 17% their skills are needed which individuals accept diversity in beliefs and lifestyles and political practices, or express conservative social values and endorse strong (i.e. 'authoritarian') government. Although the results once again offer support to those who expect differences between the two societies, the extent of divergence is relatively weak. Czechs are more socially liberal overall, but not by a great margin. Moreover, it is also evident from responses to particular items that the picture of Czech tolerance is by no means uniform. Slovaks, for example, appear more committed to freedom of religion than Czechs, though it is worth noting that in both countries support for this is overwhelming. Interesting as this result is, however, closer inspection indicates that it needs to be treated cautiously as an indicator of a more general tolerance. As we have seen, Slovaks as a group are considerably more religious than are Czechs. However, the background against which questions of religious toleration must be set is that of communist anti-religious ideology and activity. Support for religious freedom, therefore, is more likely to be an expression of the desire of the religious - emerging, as they are, from many years of communist repression - to obtain tolerance for their own which individuals accept diversity in beliefs and lifestyles and political practices, or express conservative social values and endorse strong (i.e. 'authoritarian') government. Although the results once again offer support to those who expect differences between the two societies, the extent of divergence is relatively weak. Czechs are more socially liberal overall, but not by a great margin. Moreover, it is also evident from responses to particular items that the picture of Czech tolerance is by no means uniform. Slovaks, for example, appear more committed to freedom of religion than Czechs, though it is worth noting that in both countries support for this is overwhelming. Interesting as this result is, however, closer inspection indicates that it needs to be treated cautiously as an indicator of a more general tolerance. As we have seen, Slovaks as a group are considerably more religious than are Czechs. However, the background against which questions of religious toleration must be set is that of communist anti-religious ideology and activity. Support for religious freedom, therefore, is more likely to be an expression of the desire of the religious - emerging, as they are, from many years of communist repression - to obtain tolerance for their own 141141 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 142 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS142 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS activities, than it is to reflect the broader principle of tolerance. F we do not interpret the finding as support for a counter-intuitive Slovak tolerance (nor, for obvious reasons, do we include it in the by combining the other items in the table for later analysis). None the less, even when this item is set to one side no simple pic Slovaks of both ethnicities again appear slightly more likely to agr people have no respect for traditional values; and they are less that people should be allowed to organize protest meetings against the government. But, surprisingly for a more fervently Catholic population, ethnic Slovaks are no more inclined than Czechs to be intolerant of unconventional life-styles and to favour censorship of films and magazines, and they are likely to agree that homosexuality is always wrong. Ethnic Liberalism The final dimension of political culture to be examined concerns responden willingness to include and accept the rights of ethnic minorities. Again, one se of expectations, supported by both some historical interpretations a contemporary evidence at the level of elite action, is that ethnic Slovaks are les ethnically liberal than Czechs. It is perhaps most surprising of all, therefore, t see from Table 4 how weak the grounds for this claim are. activities, than it is to reflect the broader principle of tolerance. For this reaso we do not interpret the finding as support for a counter-intuitive hypothesis Slovak tolerance (nor, for obvious reasons, do we include it in the scale formed by combining the other items in the table for later analysis). None the less, even when this item is set to one side no simple picture emerge Slovaks of both ethnicities again appear slightly more likely to agree that youn people have no respect for traditional values; and they are less likely to acc that people should be allowed to organize protest meetings against the government. But, surprisingly for a more fervently Catholic population, ethnic Slovaks are no more inclined than Czechs to be intolerant of unconventional life-styles and to favour censorship of films and magazines, and they are likely to agree that homosexuality is always wrong. Ethnic Liberalism The final dimension of political culture to be examined concerns responden willingness to include and accept the rights of ethnic minorities. Again, one se of expectations, supported by both some historical interpretations a contemporary evidence at the level of elite action, is that ethnic Slovaks are les ethnically liberal than Czechs. It is perhaps most surprising of all, therefore, t see from Table 4 how weak the grounds for this claim are. TABLE 4 Attitudes Towards Ethnic Rights by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Minority groups should Agree 8% 6% 72% have far more rights than Disagree 63% 72% 9% they do now Everyone should have the Agree 43% 76% 79% right to become a citizen Disagree 36% 11% 9% regardless of their ethnic origins The ethnic group a person Agree 63% 82% 91% belongs to should not Disagree 18% 7% 2% influence the benefits they can get from the state All minority groups Agree 76% 90% 28% should have to be taught Disagree 11% 4% 60% in (country's language) TABLE 4 Attitudes Towards Ethnic Rights by Ethnic Group Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Minority groups should Agree 8% 6% 72% have far more rights than Disagree 63% 72% 9% they do now Everyone should have the Agree 43% 76% 79% right to become a citizen Disagree 36% 11% 9% regardless of their ethnic origins The ethnic group a person Agree 63% 82% 91% belongs to should not Disagree 18% 7% 2% influence the benefits they can get from the state All minority groups Agree 76% 90% 28% should have to be taught Disagree 11% 4% 60% in (country's language) Not unexpectedly, Hungarians are vastly different in their a issues than either majority ethnic group, whereas on ave Not unexpectedly, Hungarians are vastly different in their a issues than either majority ethnic group, whereas on ave This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice between Czechs and ethnic Slovaks are only marginal - if anything, Slovaks are slightly more liberal than Czechs. On closer inspection, the distribution in the responses to specific items suggests a complex picture. Czechs and Slovaks are rather similar in the minimal support they give to the notion that ethnic minorities should have more rights - compare this with the massive level of support among Hungarians for the same proposition. Strikingly, however, Czechs are clearly less likely than ethnic Slovaks to support granting citizenship regardless of ethnic origins, or to allow people to receive state benefits without regard for their ethnic origins. Only on the question of the use of the Slovak language for teaching in schools do Slovaks exhibit clearly more illiberal responses than Czechs. The explanation of these ambiguous findings will be given further attention below. So far, we have provided evidence that there are differences as expected on all four dimensions between the two countries and between the three ethnic groups: democratic commitment and market support display relatively clear-cu differences, while social liberalism and ethnic rights have a more mixed patter Particularly with respect to democratic and market support, these differences ar firmly in the expected direction: Czechs are more liberal than Slovaks. However on each of these dimensions Slovak Hungarians also appear to be much like thei ethnically Slovak compatriots, suggesting the possibility that it is less culture than common national experience which may account for these difference Moreover, on the question of ethnic liberalism, Hungarians are predictably quit different from either group, but the direction of support for ethnic rights betwe Czechs and ethnic Slovaks is, if anything, somewhat contrary to expectations. We now turn to evidence on why these differences might have occurred. II. EVALUATING CZECH-SLOVAK DIFFERENCES IN POLITICAL AND ECONOMIC EXPERIENCE As the introduction explained in detail, one explanatio Czechs and Slovaks (and Hungarians) in their positions on the various dimensions of attitudes characterized above is that they result from longstanding national and ethnic traditions which transmit political orientations to respondents. An alternative explanation would instead emphasize variation in the current circumstances facing these groups, expressed via distinctive political and economic experiences or by the impact of differing contexts on the meaning of responses to attitude questions. The evidence above suggests support for each of these accounts: on the one hand, differences in responses are generally along the lines predicted by political culturalists; on the other, the same patterns of response could be taken to indicate the importance of current experiences and circumstances. To evaluate empirically which of these is the case, this section looks in detai at the political and economic transition experiences of our respondents in two countries. These experiences include: their evaluation of the recent performance of their country's political institutions, as well as their family's, between Czechs and ethnic Slovaks are only marginal - if anything, Slovaks are slightly more liberal than Czechs. On closer inspection, the distribution in the responses to specific items suggests a complex picture. Czechs and Slovaks are rather similar in the minimal support they give to the notion that ethnic minorities should have more rights - compare this with the massive level of support among Hungarians for the same proposition. Strikingly, however, Czechs are clearly less likely than ethnic Slovaks to support granting citizenship regardless of ethnic origins, or to allow people to receive state benefits without regard for their ethnic origins. Only on the question of the use of the Slovak language for teaching in schools do Slovaks exhibit clearly more illiberal responses than Czechs. The explanation of these ambiguous findings will be given further attention below. So far, we have provided evidence that there are differences as expected on all four dimensions between the two countries and between the three ethnic groups: democratic commitment and market support display relatively clear-cu differences, while social liberalism and ethnic rights have a more mixed patter Particularly with respect to democratic and market support, these differences ar firmly in the expected direction: Czechs are more liberal than Slovaks. However on each of these dimensions Slovak Hungarians also appear to be much like thei ethnically Slovak compatriots, suggesting the possibility that it is less culture than common national experience which may account for these difference Moreover, on the question of ethnic liberalism, Hungarians are predictably quit different from either group, but the direction of support for ethnic rights betwe Czechs and ethnic Slovaks is, if anything, somewhat contrary to expectations. We now turn to evidence on why these differences might have occurred. II. EVALUATING CZECH-SLOVAK DIFFERENCES IN POLITICAL AND ECONOMIC EXPERIENCE As the introduction explained in detail, one explanatio Czechs and Slovaks (and Hungarians) in their positions on the various dimensions of attitudes characterized above is that they result from longstanding national and ethnic traditions which transmit political orientations to respondents. An alternative explanation would instead emphasize variation in the current circumstances facing these groups, expressed via distinctive political and economic experiences or by the impact of differing contexts on the meaning of responses to attitude questions. The evidence above suggests support for each of these accounts: on the one hand, differences in responses are generally along the lines predicted by political culturalists; on the other, the same patterns of response could be taken to indicate the importance of current experiences and circumstances. To evaluate empirically which of these is the case, this section looks in detai at the political and economic transition experiences of our respondents in two countries. These experiences include: their evaluation of the recent performance of their country's political institutions, as well as their family's, 143143 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 144 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS144 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS TABLE 5 Political Experience and Involvement Among Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians TABLE 5 Political Experience and Involvement Among Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians People like me have no say in what the government does Elected officials don't care much what people like me think Government reflects the wishes of ordinary people No point in voting because government makes no difference The government acts for the benefit of the majority Everyone has an influence on the election of the government How would you evaluate the practice of democracy here? Do you think of yourself as a supporter of any particular party? People like me have no say in what the government does Elected officials don't care much what people like me think Government reflects the wishes of ordinary people No point in voting because government makes no difference The government acts for the benefit of the majority Everyone has an influence on the election of the government How would you evaluate the practice of democracy here? Do you think of yourself as a supporter of any particular party? Agree 71% 77% Disagree 14% 11% Agree 67% 79% Disagree 9% 7% Agree 71% 77% Disagree 14% 11% Agree 67% 79% Disagree 9% 7% Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree Agree Disagree 26% 40% 10% 71% 52% 44% 52% 46% 26% 40% 10% 71% 52% 44% 52% 46% 13% 63% 15% 61% 31% 65% 45% 53% 13% 63% 15% 61% 31% 65% 45% 53% Positive 40% 20% Negative 28% 43% Yes 27% 26% Positive 40% 20% Negative 28% 43% Yes 27% 26% and the country's, economic experien assessment of the effectiveness of th Examination of responses to these basis for determining whether pol account for differences on at least some of the dimensions of attitudes outlined above. It is evident from the responses to items in Table 5 that pronounced differences exist in the political experiences and evaluations of Czechs an Slovaks. Our evidence is in this respect consistent with other studies sin 1989.30 Moreover, the evidence lends credibility to the account of some nation differences at the normative level being the result of variation in politica and the country's, economic experience and prospects and their current assessment of the effectiveness of the functioning of the market econom Examination of responses to these items will provide a more systema basis for determining whether political and economic experience may account for differences on at least some of the dimensions of attitudes outlined above. It is evident from the responses to items in Table 5 that pronounced differences exist in the political experiences and evaluations of Czechs an Slovaks. Our evidence is in this respect consistent with other studies sin 1989.30 Moreover, the evidence lends credibility to the account of some nation differences at the normative level being the result of variation in politica 30 Sharon Wolchik, 'The Politics of Transition and the Break-up of Czechoslovakia', in J. Mus ed., The End of Czechoslovakia, pp. 225-44. 30 Sharon Wolchik, 'The Politics of Transition and the Break-up of Czechoslovakia', in J. Mus ed., The End of Czechoslovakia, pp. 225-44. 71% 13% 73% 7% 9% 69% 14% 61% 26% 69% 41% 53% 9% 64% 30% 71% 13% 73% 7% 9% 69% 14% 61% 26% 69% 41% 53% 9% 64% 30% This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice experience. Czechs are consistently more positive in their estimation of their input into the process of government than ethnic Slovaks and, on most items, than Slovak Hungarians, though it is also apparent that on many issues the absolute level of support for the operation of the political systems among all groups is rather low. Clearest differences emerge in response to a question that asks respondents to evaluate the 'actual practice of democracy' in the respondent's country so far, i.e. since 1989. Here, the greater incidence of elite level conflict in Slovakia is mirrored by a far greater likelihood among Slovak citizens, and not surprisingly most of all among Hungarians, to have negative views on democratic practice. Forty per cent of Czechs hold positive views about democracy in their country, compared with 20 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and only 9 per cent of Hungarians. A similar degree of difference can be seen in responses to the question of whether the government acts for the benefit of the majority in society: 52 per cent of Czechs agree with this statement compared to only 31 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 26 per cent of Hungarians. This picture of difference is matched, although sometimes to a lesser extent, across virtually all of the items in the table. Ethnic Slovaks are less likely to believe they have a say in what government does; they are much more likely than Czechs to believe that elected officials do not care much about what they think; they are more likely to think that there is no point in voting because the government cannot make any difference; and less likely to believe that they can have an influence on government. We can see, therefore, that evaluation of the political systems varies significantly between the two states in ways which may account for at least some of the national differences outlined in the previous section. A similar likelihood emerges from examination of data on the economic experiences and expectations of citizens of the two states. Table 6 presents responses to questions asking people to evaluate the direction taken in their own family's and the country's living standards over the past five years and their estimation of how family and country living standards will develop in the five years to come. On each of these items, Slovaks as a whole are more negative than Czechs, and Hungarians most negative of all. Thus, 64 per cent and 72 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians respectively believe that their family living standards have fallen over the last five years compared to only 42 per cent of Czechs. Whereas 39 per cent of Czechs expect family living standards to rise in future, only 28 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 23 per cent of Hungarians agree. Most dramatic, however, are differences in the evaluations of national economic performance. Compared to 32 per cent of Czechs who believe that the country has improved its living standards over the past five years, only 5 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 2 per cent of Hungarians believe that this has happened. Similarly, while a majority of Czechs - 55 per cent - look forward to rising standards in the country as a whole, only 30 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 22 per cent of Hungarians share this optimism. The same picture emerges when asking people to evaluate in the most general terms the performance of the experience. Czechs are consistently more positive in their estimation of their input into the process of government than ethnic Slovaks and, on most items, than Slovak Hungarians, though it is also apparent that on many issues the absolute level of support for the operation of the political systems among all groups is rather low. Clearest differences emerge in response to a question that asks respondents to evaluate the 'actual practice of democracy' in the respondent's country so far, i.e. since 1989. Here, the greater incidence of elite level conflict in Slovakia is mirrored by a far greater likelihood among Slovak citizens, and not surprisingly most of all among Hungarians, to have negative views on democratic practice. Forty per cent of Czechs hold positive views about democracy in their country, compared with 20 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and only 9 per cent of Hungarians. A similar degree of difference can be seen in responses to the question of whether the government acts for the benefit of the majority in society: 52 per cent of Czechs agree with this statement compared to only 31 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 26 per cent of Hungarians. This picture of difference is matched, although sometimes to a lesser extent, across virtually all of the items in the table. Ethnic Slovaks are less likely to believe they have a say in what government does; they are much more likely than Czechs to believe that elected officials do not care much about what they think; they are more likely to think that there is no point in voting because the government cannot make any difference; and less likely to believe that they can have an influence on government. We can see, therefore, that evaluation of the political systems varies significantly between the two states in ways which may account for at least some of the national differences outlined in the previous section. A similar likelihood emerges from examination of data on the economic experiences and expectations of citizens of the two states. Table 6 presents responses to questions asking people to evaluate the direction taken in their own family's and the country's living standards over the past five years and their estimation of how family and country living standards will develop in the five years to come. On each of these items, Slovaks as a whole are more negative than Czechs, and Hungarians most negative of all. Thus, 64 per cent and 72 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians respectively believe that their family living standards have fallen over the last five years compared to only 42 per cent of Czechs. Whereas 39 per cent of Czechs expect family living standards to rise in future, only 28 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 23 per cent of Hungarians agree. Most dramatic, however, are differences in the evaluations of national economic performance. Compared to 32 per cent of Czechs who believe that the country has improved its living standards over the past five years, only 5 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 2 per cent of Hungarians believe that this has happened. Similarly, while a majority of Czechs - 55 per cent - look forward to rising standards in the country as a whole, only 30 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and 22 per cent of Hungarians share this optimism. The same picture emerges when asking people to evaluate in the most general terms the performance of the 145145 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 146 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS146 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS TABLE 6 Economic Experience and Expectations Among Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Living standards of Risen 27% 13% 8% family over past five years Fallen 42% 64% 72% Living standards of Will rise 39% 28% 23% family over next five years Will fall 20% 33% 35% Living standards of Risen 32% 5% 2% country over past five years Fallen 47% 87% 92% Living standards of Will rise 55% 30% 22% country over next five years Will fall 13% 33% 47% How would you evaluate Positive 38% 16% 13% the market economy so far? Negative 29% 52% 62% TABLE 6 Economic Experience and Expectations Among Czechs, Slovaks and Slovak Hungarians Slovak Czechs Slovaks Hungarians Living standards of Risen 27% 13% 8% family over past five years Fallen 42% 64% 72% Living standards of Will rise 39% 28% 23% family over next five years Will fall 20% 33% 35% Living standards of Risen 32% 5% 2% country over past five years Fallen 47% 87% 92% Living standards of Will rise 55% 30% 22% country over next five years Will fall 13% 33% 47% How would you evaluate Positive 38% 16% 13% the market economy so far? Negative 29% 52% 62% market economy so far: 38 per cent of Czechs are positive, compa 16 per cent and 13 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians. The evidence about economic experience adds to that concernin evaluations: while we cannot yet rule out the culturalist explana differences in orientations towards the normative items described in Section I, there is at least a strong primafacie case for the importance of recent experience in explaining differences. The next section seeks to provide a stricter test of the explanatory power of each of these types of account. III. EXPLAINING DIFFERENCES IN ATTITUDES BETWEEN CZECHS, SLOVAKS AND SLOVAK HUNGARIANS In this section we attempt to decide between political cu sorts of explanations of the country and group d Section I. Our strategy is to regress measures of dem economic and social liberalism on the measures of politic experience and evaluation. The aim is to try and acc observed differences between Czechs and Slovaks in levels of commitment to democracy, the market and social liberalism. We do this by first entering variables which represent the effect of being in a particular country or ethnic group (so-called 'dummy' variables), into a regression model and then adding potential explanatory variables. If these explanatory variables account for the observed national differences, then their addition to the regression models should reduce the coefficients for country differences to statistically insignificant levels. market economy so far: 38 per cent of Czechs are positive, compared to only 16 per cent and 13 per cent of ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians. The evidence about economic experience adds to that concerning political evaluations: while we cannot yet rule out the culturalist explanation of differences in orientations towards the normative items described in Section I, there is at least a strong primafacie case for the importance of recent experience in explaining differences. The next section seeks to provide a stricter test of the explanatory power of each of these types of account. III. EXPLAINING DIFFERENCES IN ATTITUDES BETWEEN CZECHS, SLOVAKS AND SLOVAK HUNGARIANS In this section we attempt to decide between political cu sorts of explanations of the country and group d Section I. Our strategy is to regress measures of dem economic and social liberalism on the measures of politic experience and evaluation. The aim is to try and acc observed differences between Czechs and Slovaks in levels of commitment to democracy, the market and social liberalism. We do this by first entering variables which represent the effect of being in a particular country or ethnic group (so-called 'dummy' variables), into a regression model and then adding potential explanatory variables. If these explanatory variables account for the observed national differences, then their addition to the regression models should reduce the coefficients for country differences to statistically insignificant levels. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice For example, we shall assess the extent to which individual differences in levels of democratic commitment between groups are removed by controllin for individual differences in the independent variables - the indicators of political and economic experience - and the relationship between these and nationality.31 Where we succeed in removing group differences by this method, it provides evidence that recent experience and evaluations of the workings of the political and economic system can explain variations in commitment, rather than long-standing national traditions as expressed through common normative orientations. However, where country differences are not removed in this way, or are removed only by reference to variables that can be taken to represen long-standing national traditions, we find evidence consistent with cultura forms of explanation.32 The dependent variables used in these analyses are constructed from responses to groups of questions which have been combined to form summated rating ('Likert') scales. In a Likert scale, responses to the constituent items are given scores (ranging, for example, from 1 through to 5). These scores are then simply added together. It is assumed that each item is a parallel measure of the same underlying concept (although each may tap slightly different aspects o it). Because each item may contain considerable measurement error and/o specificity, a strength of Likert scaling is that it does not give too great a importance to any particular item. The internal consistency of the scales wa assessed using Cronbach's alpha, which is an estimate of reliability related to the average inter-item correlation. Selection of scale items was done partly on a priori grounds, but items that were found to reduce internal consistency (as measured by alpha) were removed. This was not done, however, with some items that helped preserve the balance of the scales with regard to direction of question wording. These were retained even when their presence reduced the internal consistency of the scales (particularly the social liberalism scale), because balanced scales are less prone to the biases associated with acquiescence effects.33 31 For an elaboration of this approach to cross-national analysis, the aim of which is to replace country names with theoretically relevant variables, see Adam Przeworski and H. Teune, The Logic of Comparative Social Enquiry (New York: Wiley, 1970) An illustration of this approach, also using individual level data, can be found in Evans and Whitefield, 'The Politics and Economics of Democratic Commitment'. 32 We also conducted analyses which included relatively stable national attributes such as the distribution of respondents across different social classes, educational levels and urban-rural residence. However, these social structural characteristics failed to make any contribution to explaining country differences (details available on request). 33 For similar treatments of acquiescence effects in comparable data to that presented here, see Anthony Heath, Geoffrey Evans and J. Martin, 'The Measurement of Core Beliefs and Values: The Development of Balanced Socialist/Laissez-Faire and Libertarian/Authoritarian Scales', British Journal of Political Science, 24 (1994), 115-32. For an assessment of the consequences of the presence, or otherwise, of balanced question wording in scales, see Geoffrey Evans and Anthony Heath, 'The Measurement of Left-Right and Libertarian-Authoritarian Values: Comparing Balanced and Unbalanced Scales', Quality and Quantity, 29 (1995), 191-206. For example, we shall assess the extent to which individual differences in levels of democratic commitment between groups are removed by controlling for individual differences in the independent variables - the indicators of political and economic experience - and the relationship between these and nationality.31 Where we succeed in removing group differences by this method, it provides evidence that recent experience and evaluations of the workings of the political and economic system can explain variations in commitment, rather than long-standing national traditions as expressed through common normative orientations. However, where country differences are not removed in this way, or are removed only by reference to variables that can be taken to represent long-standing national traditions, we find evidence consistent with cultural forms of explanation.32 The dependent variables used in these analyses are constructed from responses to groups of questions which have been combined to form summated rating ('Likert') scales. In a Likert scale, responses to the constituent items are given scores (ranging, for example, from 1 through to 5). These scores are then simply added together. It is assumed that each item is a parallel measure of the same underlying concept (although each may tap slightly different aspects of it). Because each item may contain considerable measurement error and/or specificity, a strength of Likert scaling is that it does not give too great an importance to any particular item. The internal consistency of the scales was assessed using Cronbach's alpha, which is an estimate of reliability related to the average inter-item correlation. Selection of scale items was done partly on a priori grounds, but items that were found to reduce internal consistency (as measured by alpha) were removed. This was not done, however, with some items that helped preserve the balance of the scales with regard to direction of question wording. These were retained even when their presence reduced the internal consistency of the scales (particularly the social liberalism scale), because balanced scales are less prone to the biases associated with acquiescence effects.33 31 For an elaboration of this approach to cross-national analysis, the aim of which is to replace country names with theoretically relevant variables, see Adam Przeworski and H. Teune, The Logic of Comparative Social Enquiry (New York: Wiley, 1970) An illustration of this approach, also using individual level data, can be found in Evans and Whitefield, 'The Politics and Economics of Democratic Commitment'. 32 We also conducted analyses which included relatively stable national attributes such as the distribution of respondents across different social classes, educational levels and urban-rural residence. However, these social structural characteristics failed to make any contribution to explaining country differences (details available on request). 33 For similar treatments of acquiescence effects in comparable data to that presented here, see Anthony Heath, Geoffrey Evans and J. Martin, 'The Measurement of Core Beliefs and Values: The Development of Balanced Socialist/Laissez-Faire and Libertarian/Authoritarian Scales', British Journal of Political Science, 24 (1994), 115-32. For an assessment of the consequences of the presence, or otherwise, of balanced question wording in scales, see Geoffrey Evans and Anthony Heath, 'The Measurement of Left-Right and Libertarian-Authoritarian Values: Comparing Balanced and Unbalanced Scales', Quality and Quantity, 29 (1995), 191-206. 147147 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 148 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS148 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS Democratic commitment is measured using the four items Cronbach's alpha is 0.64 in the Czech Republic and 0.63 in Slov liberalism is measured using the ten items presented in Table alpha is 0.77 in the Czech Republic and 0.71 in Slovakia. Social includes seven of the items in Table 3. Cronbach's alpha is 0.46 Republic and 0.42 in Slovakia. The political experience scale cont six items shown in Table 6. Cronbach's alpha was 0.67 in the C and 0.58 in Slovakia. All of the items used in the analyses are coded scores indicate liberal positions or positive evaluations of political and economic practice. Scores on items which are inconsistent with this direction have been reversed. 'Don't know' responses are recoded to the mid-point of the scale.34 In Table 7 we model differences between Czechs, ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians with respect to the democratic commitment scale. The first model, which includes only the dummy variables for country and ethnic group, restates the differences in mean scores shown in Table 1; both ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians are significantly different from the somewhat more liberal Czechs. Models 2 and 3 seek to remove these differences by introducing the political experience scale and measures of economic experience examined in the previous section. TABLE 7 Democratic Commitment Regressed on Country, Political and Economic Experience t Model 1: Model 2: Model 3: Ethnic and Political Economic country dummies experience experience Czech Republic Slovaks - 0.18** -0.04** 0.00 Hungarians - 0.07** 0.02 0.04 Political experience 0.43** Democracy evaluation 0.22** Family past 0.15** Family future 0.04 Country past 0.02 Country future 0.17** Market evaluation 0.27** Adjusted r2 0.03 0.32 0.22 **Significant of p < 0.01. tThe values shown are standardized beta coef 34 If 'Don't knows' are treated as missing it re on the substantive findings. Details available Democratic commitment is measu Cronbach's alpha is 0.64 in the Czec liberalism is measured using the te alpha is 0.77 in the Czech Republic includes seven of the items in Tabl Republic and 0.42 in Slovakia. The p six items shown in Table 6. Cronba and 0.58 in Slovakia. All of the items scores indicate liberal positions or positive evaluations of political and economic practice. Scores on items which are inconsistent with this direction have been reversed. 'Don't know' responses are recoded to the mid-point of the scale.34 In Table 7 we model differences between Czechs, ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians with respect to the democratic commitment scale. The first model, which includes only the dummy variables for country and ethnic group, restates the differences in mean scores shown in Table 1; both ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians are significantly different from the somewhat more liberal Czechs. Models 2 and 3 seek to remove these differences by introducing the political experience scale and measures of economic experience examined in the previous section. TABLE 7 Democratic Commitment Regressed on Country, Political and Economic Experience t Model 1: Model 2: Model 3: Ethnic and Political Economic country dummies experience experience Czech Republic Slovaks - 0.18** -0.04** 0.00 Hungarians - 0.07** 0.02 0.04 Political experience 0.43** Democracy evaluation 0.22** Family past 0.15** Family future 0.04 Country past 0.02 Country future 0.17** Market evaluation 0.27** Adjusted r2 0.03 0.32 0.22 **Significant of p < 0.01. tThe values shown are standardized beta coef 34 If 'Don't knows' are treated as missing it re on the substantive findings. Details available This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice In Model 2, the political experience scale and the single item on how respondents evaluate the actual practice of democracy in their country so far are introduced as independent variables. As can be seen, both of these have a significant and substantial impact on levels of democratic commitment, with standardized beta coefficients of 0.43 and 0.22. The effect of the introduction of these variables is virtually to remove most of the differences between ethnic Slovaks and Czechs with respect to democratic commitment - though still statistically significant, the beta coefficient for Slovaks falls from -0.18 to - 0.04. Differences between Hungarians and Czechs are removed completely. The r2 for Model 2 is also increased over that of Model 1 from 0.03 to a substantial 0.32. Model 3 introduces the economic experience variables. Once again, a numb of these variables are highly significant statistically - family living standards over the past five years, country living standards over the next five an evaluation of the market in practice are strongly associated with democrat commitment - and differences in the country or group variables are removed entirely. The effects of economic experience are even more effective than thos of political experience at accounting for country differences, primarily becaus differences in economic experience between the two countries are considerably more marked than those in political experience.35 We can thus account for higher levels of commitment to democracy among Czechs by comparison with Slovaks without recourse to notions of long-standin political culture. Higher levels of support for democracy in the Czech Republic appear to reflect that country' s greater success in making the economic transitio resulting in materially less harsh market experiences, and its relatively straightforward democratic transition. These have resulted in less elite conflict and a greater willingness to allow citizens say in political development than has occurred in the more highly fractious political disputes found in Slovakia. Culturalists might still want to argue that the political experience items are themselves conditioned by culture rather than experience. Indeed, Almond and Verba and others have used similar items as part of their 'civic cultur measures. There are, however, several replies to this point: first, at face value these items clearly ask about the workings of the political system as experience by respondents; we therefore see no good reason to treat these responses reflections of cultural differences. The patterns of Czech, Slovak and Hungarian responses make good sense as reflections of differences in the party systems in 35 The importance of economic experiences in accounting for differences in democratic attitud between Czechs and Slovaks is slightly at odds with our analysis of other countries in the regio where political experience was of clearly greater importance in explaining national variation support for democracy than was economic experience. The reasons for this divergence probably l in the particularly positive experience of the market reported by Czechs compared with all oth countries we have surveyed in Eastern Europe, and the fact that until 1993 the political system experienced by Czechs and Slovaks were the same, thus reducing the extent of experience differences in political experience and evaluation. See Evans and Whitefield, 'The Politics an Economics of Democratic Commitment'. In Model 2, the political experience scale and the single item on how respondents evaluate the actual practice of democracy in their country so far are introduced as independent variables. As can be seen, both of these have a significant and substantial impact on levels of democratic commitment, with standardized beta coefficients of 0.43 and 0.22. The effect of the introduction of these variables is virtually to remove most of the differences between ethnic Slovaks and Czechs with respect to democratic commitment - though still statistically significant, the beta coefficient for Slovaks falls from -0.18 to - 0.04. Differences between Hungarians and Czechs are removed completely. The r2 for Model 2 is also increased over that of Model 1 from 0.03 to a substantial 0.32. Model 3 introduces the economic experience variables. Once again, a numb of these variables are highly significant statistically - family living standards over the past five years, country living standards over the next five an evaluation of the market in practice are strongly associated with democrat commitment - and differences in the country or group variables are removed entirely. The effects of economic experience are even more effective than thos of political experience at accounting for country differences, primarily becaus differences in economic experience between the two countries are considerably more marked than those in political experience.35 We can thus account for higher levels of commitment to democracy among Czechs by comparison with Slovaks without recourse to notions of long-standin political culture. Higher levels of support for democracy in the Czech Republic appear to reflect that country' s greater success in making the economic transitio resulting in materially less harsh market experiences, and its relatively straightforward democratic transition. These have resulted in less elite conflict and a greater willingness to allow citizens say in political development than has occurred in the more highly fractious political disputes found in Slovakia. Culturalists might still want to argue that the political experience items are themselves conditioned by culture rather than experience. Indeed, Almond and Verba and others have used similar items as part of their 'civic cultur measures. There are, however, several replies to this point: first, at face value these items clearly ask about the workings of the political system as experience by respondents; we therefore see no good reason to treat these responses reflections of cultural differences. The patterns of Czech, Slovak and Hungarian responses make good sense as reflections of differences in the party systems in 35 The importance of economic experiences in accounting for differences in democratic attitud between Czechs and Slovaks is slightly at odds with our analysis of other countries in the regio where political experience was of clearly greater importance in explaining national variation support for democracy than was economic experience. The reasons for this divergence probably l in the particularly positive experience of the market reported by Czechs compared with all oth countries we have surveyed in Eastern Europe, and the fact that until 1993 the political system experienced by Czechs and Slovaks were the same, thus reducing the extent of experience differences in political experience and evaluation. See Evans and Whitefield, 'The Politics an Economics of Democratic Commitment'. 149149 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 150 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS150 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS the two countries and the representation available to the various et If political culturalists want to assume these responses reflect cultur then they need to make a good case for that assumption. Secondly, in democratic commitment between Czechs and Slovaks can be acco economic experience without including measures of political exp regression model (this is because economic and political ex themselves strongly correlated). Thirdly, if we do treat the indica experience as dependent rather than independent variables in our a as a cultural phenomenon to be explained rather than as an explana responses - we find that differences in responses between Czechs a wholly explained by economic experience (details available). It is not surprising, therefore, that a similar analytic strategy is in removing differences in the levels of market commitment amo groups which emerged from Table 2. Again, Model 1 in Table 8 that ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians are both significantly les committed to the market than are Czechs. However, when the item Table 6 are introduced into the second model as independent v prove to have substantial effects. The economic experience of t the past five years and a general evaluation of the market in pract being particularly important in reducing differences between country dummy variables, the beta coefficients for which fall fro ethnic Slovaks to - 0.04 (barely significant) and from - 0.11 to case of Hungarians. Again, this is consistent with the proposition t in levels of normative commitment to liberalization - in this case to the economy - among the three groups is not the result o long-standing traditions and orientations but the consequence of t the two states to manage the market transition successfully. TABLE 8 Economic Liberalism Regressed on Country and Economic Experience (OLS Regressions) t Model 1: Model 2: Ethnic and country dumm Czech Republic Slovaks -0.21** -0.02 Hungarians -0.11** 0.00 Family past - 0.19** Family future - 0.08** Country past - 0.13** Country future - 0.10** Market evaluation - 0.16** Adjusted r2 0.04 0.22 *Significant at p <0.05. **Significant coefficients. the two countries and the representation available to the various ethnic groups. If political culturalists want to assume these responses reflect cultural dispositions then they need to make a good case for that assumption. Secondly, differences in democratic commitment between Czechs and Slovaks can be accounted for by economic experience without including measures of political experience in the regression model (this is because economic and political experience are themselves strongly correlated). Thirdly, if we do treat the indicators of political experience as dependent rather than independent variables in our analysis - i.e. as a cultural phenomenon to be explained rather than as an explanation of such responses - we find that differences in responses between Czechs and Slovaks are wholly explained by economic experience (details available). It is not surprising, therefore, that a similar analytic strategy is also effective in removing differences in the levels of market commitment among the three groups which emerged from Table 2. Again, Model 1 in Table 8 demonstrates that ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians are both significantly less normatively committed to the market than are Czechs. However, when the items detailed in Table 6 are introduced into the second model as independent variables, they prove to have substantial effects. The economic experience of the family over the past five years and a general evaluation of the market in practice emerge as being particularly important in reducing differences between the group and country dummy variables, the beta coefficients for which fall from - 0.21 for ethnic Slovaks to - 0.04 (barely significant) and from - 0.11 to - 0.01 in the case of Hungarians. Again, this is consistent with the proposition that variations in levels of normative commitment to liberalization - in this case with respect to the economy - among the three groups is not the result of different long-standing traditions and orientations but the consequence of the capacity of the two states to manage the market transition successfully. TABLE 8 Economic Liberalism Regressed on Country and Economic Experience (OLS Regressions) t Model 1: Model 2: Ethnic and country dumm Czech Republic Slovaks -0.21** -0.02 Hungarians -0.11** 0.00 Family past - 0.19** Family future - 0.08** Country past - 0.13** Country future - 0.10** Market evaluation - 0.16** Adjusted r2 0.04 0.22 *Significant at p <0.05. **Significant coefficients. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice The same argument does not, however, explain the (admittedly smaller) differences among the groups on questions of social liberalism (shown in Table 3). The introduction of political and economic experience and evaluation as independent variables in regression analyses (details available on request) makes no dent in removing differences in the country or group dummies - in this case, in removing the relative social illiberalism of ethnic Slovaks. Ther is little reason for believing, therefore, that differences in levels of social liberalism result from recent experience, individual resource endowments, or evaluation of performance and prospects. Rather, as Table 9 shows, these differences between the country or groups are only removed when variation in levels of church attendance is controlled for. As we have seen, Slovaks are clearly more religious than are Czechs. This fact, which derives from long-standing differences in the two countries' historical traditions, appears best to explain the lower levels of commitment to social liberalism among Slovaks. In this case, we conclude that differences in the two political cultures - reflecting the different historical role of Catholicism in the two countries - is the more likely explanation for small differences which emerge in responses to ou questions about individual liberties, the right to protest, and tolerance of differences in lifestyles. TABLE 9 Social Liberalism Regressed on Country, Religious Denomination and Levels of Church Attendance (OLS Regressions) t Model 1: Model 2: Ethnic and country dummi Czech Republic Slovaks - 0.12** - 0.03 Hungarians - 0.04* 0.01 No religion 0.03 Church attendance - 0.18** Adjusted r2 0.01 0.04 *Significant at p <0.05. **Significa standardized beta coefficients. To summarize: Czech-Slovak differences with respect to the major issues of economic and political liberalization appear to be the result of recent experience, resources, evaluations and circumstances rather than deeper normative orientations. However, attitudes towards various social issues involving questions of liberal rights and tolerance of disagreement and diversity do not seem to relate to economic or political experience, but rather to long-standing traditions and identities of the two states as reflected in participation in the country's religious institutions. The same argument does not, however, explain the (admittedly smaller) differences among the groups on questions of social liberalism (shown in Table 3). The introduction of political and economic experience and evaluation as independent variables in regression analyses (details available on request) makes no dent in removing differences in the country or group dummies - in this case, in removing the relative social illiberalism of ethnic Slovaks. There is little reason for believing, therefore, that differences in levels of social liberalism result from recent experience, individual resource endowments, or evaluation of performance and prospects. Rather, as Table 9 shows, these differences between the country or groups are only removed when variation in levels of church attendance is controlled for. As we have seen, Slovaks are clearly more religious than are Czechs. This fact, which derives from long-standing differences in the two countries' historical traditions, appears best to explain the lower levels of commitment to social liberalism among Slovaks. In this case, we conclude that differences in the two political cultures - reflecting the different historical role of Catholicism in the two countries - is the more likely explanation for small differences which emerge in responses to ou questions about individual liberties, the right to protest, and tolerance of differences in lifestyles. TABLE 9 Social Liberalism Regressed on Country, Religious Denomination and Levels of Church Attendance (OLS Regressions) t Model 1: Model 2: Ethnic and country dummi Czech Republic Slovaks - 0.12** - 0.03 Hungarians - 0.04* 0.01 No religion 0.03 Church attendance - 0.18** Adjusted r2 0.01 0.04 *Significant at p <0.05. **Significa standardized beta coefficients. To summarize: Czech-Slovak differences with respect to the major issues of economic and political liberalization appear to be the result of recent experience, resources, evaluations and circumstances rather than deeper normative orientations. However, attitudes towards various social issues involving questions of liberal rights and tolerance of disagreement and diversity do not seem to relate to economic or political experience, but rather to long-standing traditions and identities of the two states as reflected in participation in the country's religious institutions. 151151 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 152 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS152 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS IV. THE MEANING OF ETHNIC LIBERALISM It remains only to return to an account of the unexp Czechs and Slovaks on questions of ethnic liberalis attitudes towards ethnic rights are more complex. items (ethnic rights and language in schools), Slov slightly - with the latter tending to be the slightly m while Hungarians are clearly far more pro-minorit items, however, it is Czechs who appear to be marked while Hungarians lose much of their liberal distin focus on the rights to citizenship and state benefits o At first glance this inconsistent pattern may seem removed by a regression analysis similar to the o available on request). It seems likely, however, that ex of responses results from differences in the meaning the ethnic rights questions for Czechs and Slovaks ethnic rights elicit considerations about Hungarian have no such similarly large and distinctive group wi connect more distinctly to the gypsy question, and p the case of citizenship) to the rights of ethnic German of Bohemia and Moravia after the end of the Second World War. These considerations make salient questions about the rights of gypsies (and possi Germans) to be citizens of the Czech Republic. These differences of interpret may therefore explain the substantial proportion of Czechs who react some negatively to these issues. For Slovaks, in contrast, the rights of Hungarian be citizens and to have rights to state benefits are not such sensitive topics These considerations seem to be borne out by differences in the patterns association between attitudes towards ethnic issues in the two countries. In addition to the four questions shown in Table 4, respondents were also asked about their beliefs concerning the presence of 'too many' gypsies in their countries.36 For Czechs, attitudes towards gypsies predict attitudes towards citizenship rights (r = 0.20, significant at p < 0.01), but for Slovaks they do not (r = - 0.02), ns. Similarly for Czechs, but not for Slovaks, beliefs about ethnic rights in general are moderately strongly associated with attitudes towards citizenship rights (r = 0.27, p < 0.01) - for Slovaks there is only a very weak association (R = 0.11, p < 0.01), whereas the link between ethnic rights in general and attitudes towards the use of the majority language for teaching in schools is very strong in Slovakia (0.54, p <0.01) but not (0.20, p < 0.01) among Czechs. Finally, there is evidence that attitudes towards the use of the majority language for teaching are more closely linked with attitudes towards national independence37 among Slovaks (0.25, p < 0.01) than they are among 36 'There are too many gypsies in the Czech Republic/Slovakia.' Both ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians were more likely than Czechs to agree with this statement - perhaps because there ar more gypsies in Slovakia than in the Czech Republic. 37 'The Czech Republic/Slovakia should co-operate with other countries even if it means giving up some independence.' IV. THE MEANING OF ETHNIC LIBERALISM It remains only to return to an account of the unexp Czechs and Slovaks on questions of ethnic liberalis attitudes towards ethnic rights are more complex. items (ethnic rights and language in schools), Slov slightly - with the latter tending to be the slightly m while Hungarians are clearly far more pro-minorit items, however, it is Czechs who appear to be marked while Hungarians lose much of their liberal distin focus on the rights to citizenship and state benefits o At first glance this inconsistent pattern may seem removed by a regression analysis similar to the o available on request). It seems likely, however, that ex of responses results from differences in the meaning the ethnic rights questions for Czechs and Slovaks ethnic rights elicit considerations about Hungarian have no such similarly large and distinctive group wi connect more distinctly to the gypsy question, and p the case of citizenship) to the rights of ethnic German of Bohemia and Moravia after the end of the Second World War. These considerations make salient questions about the rights of gypsies (and possi Germans) to be citizens of the Czech Republic. These differences of interpret may therefore explain the substantial proportion of Czechs who react some negatively to these issues. For Slovaks, in contrast, the rights of Hungarian be citizens and to have rights to state benefits are not such sensitive topics These considerations seem to be borne out by differences in the patterns association between attitudes towards ethnic issues in the two countries. In addition to the four questions shown in Table 4, respondents were also asked about their beliefs concerning the presence of 'too many' gypsies in their countries.36 For Czechs, attitudes towards gypsies predict attitudes towards citizenship rights (r = 0.20, significant at p < 0.01), but for Slovaks they do not (r = - 0.02), ns. Similarly for Czechs, but not for Slovaks, beliefs about ethnic rights in general are moderately strongly associated with attitudes towards citizenship rights (r = 0.27, p < 0.01) - for Slovaks there is only a very weak association (R = 0.11, p < 0.01), whereas the link between ethnic rights in general and attitudes towards the use of the majority language for teaching in schools is very strong in Slovakia (0.54, p <0.01) but not (0.20, p < 0.01) among Czechs. Finally, there is evidence that attitudes towards the use of the majority language for teaching are more closely linked with attitudes towards national independence37 among Slovaks (0.25, p < 0.01) than they are among 36 'There are too many gypsies in the Czech Republic/Slovakia.' Both ethnic Slovaks and Hungarians were more likely than Czechs to agree with this statement - perhaps because there ar more gypsies in Slovakia than in the Czech Republic. 37 'The Czech Republic/Slovakia should co-operate with other countries even if it means giving up some independence.' This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Political Culture Versus Rational ChoicePolitical Culture Versus Rational Choice Czechs (0.04, ns); a similar tendency can be observed with respect to ethnic rights in general among Slovaks (0.32 and 0.17, respectively (both p < 0.01)). The issues made salient with respect to the ethnic rights question do appear to differ in the two countries, just as the groups under consideration appear to differ. Among Czechs the link is between ethnic rights, gypsies and citizenship. Among Slovaks, beliefs about ethnic rights do not link even moderately strongly with views on citizenship - probably because this is not a right from which ethnically illiberal Slovaks want to exclude the Hungarian minority (if anything, this would facilitate the possibility of secession). Similarly, because gypsies are not the group brought most obviously to mind by the question of ethnic minority rights, there is no link among Slovaks between attitudes towards citizenship rights and beliefs about the presence of undesirably large numbers of gypsies; while there are stronger links between ethnic rights and both attitudes towards national independence and the issue of language in schools among Slovaks than among Czechs. By illustrating the different connotations of ethnic rights questions in the two countries this evidence suggests, once again, that differences in political culture conceived as general normative orientations to minority rights are not the key to understanding differences in Czech-Slovak political attitudes. What matters are differences in the circumstances faced by these two populations resulting from the presence or otherwise of challenges to the nation state and the character of the ethnic groups to be incorporated in them. These factors shift the meaning of questions about minority rights in ways which make comprehensible otherwise surprising results. V. CONCLUSION Our strategy in this article has been to test for the relati cultural versus rational choice explanations of national attitudes. We cannot claim to have tested the relative m conclusively - and in any case, only one way of conceiv explanation has been dealt with. None the less, much suggests that political culture is of less importance tha expectations and the country's ethnic, political and eco conditioning Czech-Slovak differences in political attit Inevitably, when comparing answers to attitude ques contexts there has to be considerable sensitivity to p measurement and meaning - indeed, such a difference ethnic liberalism has been identified and elucidated a however, the case of the Czech Republic and Slovakia appeared to offer particularly fruitful ground for compari the fact that the surveys were conducted by the same contaminating effects resulting from differing 'hou linguistic comparability, and many shared nuances frequencies more directly comparable than is often the Czechs (0.04, ns); a similar tendency can be observed w rights in general among Slovaks (0.32 and 0.17, respect The issues made salient with respect to the ethnic rights to differ in the two countries, just as the groups under c differ. Among Czechs the link is between ethnic rights, g Among Slovaks, beliefs about ethnic rights do not link ev with views on citizenship - probably because this is no ethnically illiberal Slovaks want to exclude the Hungarian this would facilitate the possibility of secession). Similarly not the group brought most obviously to mind by the ques rights, there is no link among Slovaks between attitud rights and beliefs about the presence of undesirably large while there are stronger links between ethnic rights and national independence and the issue of language in schools among Czechs. By illustrating the different connotations of ethnic right countries this evidence suggests, once again, that differen conceived as general normative orientations to minority r to understanding differences in Czech-Slovak political atti are differences in the circumstances faced by these tw from the presence or otherwise of challenges to the nation of the ethnic groups to be incorporated in them. These fact of questions about minority rights in ways which m otherwise surprising results. V. CONCLUSION Our strategy in this article has been to test for the relati cultural versus rational choice explanations of national attitudes. We cannot claim to have tested the relative m conclusively - and in any case, only one way of conceiv explanation has been dealt with. None the less, much suggests that political culture is of less importance tha expectations and the country's ethnic, political and eco conditioning Czech-Slovak differences in political attit Inevitably, when comparing answers to attitude ques contexts there has to be considerable sensitivity to p measurement and meaning - indeed, such a difference ethnic liberalism has been identified and elucidated a however, the case of the Czech Republic and Slovakia appeared to offer particularly fruitful ground for compari the fact that the surveys were conducted by the same contaminating effects resulting from differing 'hou linguistic comparability, and many shared nuances frequencies more directly comparable than is often the 153153 This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 154 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS154 WHITEFIELD AND EVANS comparisons. We can have some confidence, therefore, that the d observed between political attitudes in the two countries are substant than artefactual. Given this, we consider it is reasonable to conclude that on a number of dimensions of attitudes where differences between Czechs and Slovaks - and Hungarians - were found, and which could have been explained in terms o enduring systems of belief, they could be shown empirically to be associated with differences in transition experiences. If parsimony and testability are to be given weight when comparing explanatory accounts, there seems little reason in these cases to have recourse to the complexities of political culture. True, rational choice does not account for all of the observed differences between Czechs and Slovaks. Those concerning social liberalism appear to relate only to frequency of church attendance - and the place of this institution, and the attachment of Slovaks to it, is best understood as part of the long-standing traditions and identities of Slovak culture. In this sense, political culture explanation has a role; indeed, it is precisely the role that Almond, in his retrospective appraisal of the value of political culture theory, is most secure in ascribing to it. As he puts it, 'most resistant to change are attitudes, identities and value commitments associated with ethnicity, nationality and religion'.38 Our evidence supports this. None the less, even this attitudinal difference was muted and not consistent across issues. Indeed, of most interest with respect to issues of social liberalism is how little difference there is between two communities with such marked differences in religiosity and relations w Catholic Church. Thus even this finding might be taken as evidence of h this most enduring of cultural differences between Czechs and Slovaks is explanation of their current political beliefs. comparisons. We can have some confidence, therefore, that the diff observed between political attitudes in the two countries are substantive than artefactual. Given this, we consider it is reasonable to conclude that on a number of dimensions of attitudes where differences between Czechs and Slovaks - and Hungarians - were found, and which could have been explained in terms o enduring systems of belief, they could be shown empirically to be associated with differences in transition experiences. If parsimony and testability are to be given weight when comparing explanatory accounts, there seems little reason in these cases to have recourse to the complexities of political culture. True, rational choice does not account for all of the observed differences between Czechs and Slovaks. Those concerning social liberalism appear to relate only to frequency of church attendance - and the place of this institution, and the attachment of Slovaks to it, is best understood as part of the long-standing traditions and identities of Slovak culture. In this sense, political culture explanation has a role; indeed, it is precisely the role that Almond, in his retrospective appraisal of the value of political culture theory, is most secure in ascribing to it. As he puts it, 'most resistant to change are attitudes, identities and value commitments associated with ethnicity, nationality and religion'.38 Our evidence supports this. None the less, even this attitudinal difference was muted and not consistent across issues. Indeed, of most interest with respect to issues of social liberalism is how little difference there is between two communities with such marked differences in religiosity and relations w Catholic Church. Thus even this finding might be taken as evidence of h this most enduring of cultural differences between Czechs and Slovaks is explanation of their current political beliefs. 38 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 22.38 Almond, 'The Study of Political Culture', p. 22. This content downloaded from 147.251.110.15 on Mon, 20 Feb 2017 13:01:57 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms