THE PRESENTATION OF SELF 1 IN EVERYDAY LIFE ERVING GOFFMAN University of Edinburgh Social Sciences Research Centre Price : Ten Shillings THE PRESENTATION OF SELF IN EVERYDAY LIFE ERVING GOFFMAN University of Edinburgh Social Sciences Research Centre $9 George Square, Edinburgh S Monograph No. 2 1956 o M asks are arrested ex p ressio n s and admirable echoes of feeling, at once faithful, discreet, and superlative. Living th ings in contact with the air must acquire a cuticle, and it is not urged against cu ticles that they are not h ea rts; yet some philosophers seem to be angry with im ages for not being things, and with words for not being feelings. Words and images are like sh ells, no le s s integral p arts of nature than are the substa n c e s they cover, but better addressed to the eye and more open to observation. I would not say that substance e x is ts for the sake of appearance, or faces for the sake of masks, or the p a s sio n s for the sake of poetry and virtue. Nothing a rise s in nature for the sak e of anything e l s e ; all these p h a s e s and products are involved equally in the round of e x i s t e n c e ............. George Santayana 1 1Soliloquies in E ngland and L ater S o lilo q u ies (New York: S cribners, 1922), pp. 131-132. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The report presented here w as developed in connection with a study of interaction undertaken for the Department of Social Anthropology and the Social S ciences Research Committee of the U niversity of Edinburgh and a study of social stratification supported by a Ford Foundation grant directed by P rofessor E. A. S hils at the U niversity of Chicago. I am grateful to th ese so u rc e s of guidance and support. I would like to express thanks to my teac h ers C. W. M .Hart, W. L. Warner, and E. C. Hughes. [ want, too, to thank E lizabeth Bott, Ja m es Littlejohn, and Edward Banfield, who helped me at the beginning of the study, and fellow -students of occupations at the University of Chicago who helped me later. Without the collaboration of my wife, Angelica S. Goffman, th is report would not have been written. PREFA CE 1 mean th is report to serve a s a sort of handbook detailing one sociological perspective from which social life can be studied, esp ecially the kind of social life thar is organised within the physical confines of a building or plant. A set of features will be described which together form a framework that can be applied to any concrete social establishm ent, be it domestic, industrial, or commercial. The perspective employed in this report is that of the theatrical performance ; the principles derived are dramaturgical ones. I shall consider the way in which the individual in ordinary work situations presents him self and his activity to others, the ways in which he guides and controls the im pression they form of him, and the kinds of things he may and may not do while sustaining his performance before them. In using th is model I will attempt not to make light of its obvious inadequacies. The stage p resen ts things that are make-believe ; presumably life p resen ts things that are real and som etim es not well rehearsed. More important, perhaps, on the stage one player »resents himself in the guise of a cha^.cter to ch aracters projected by other p lay ers; the audience co n stitu tes a third party to the interaction—one that is essen tial and yet, if the stage performance were real, one that would not be there. In real life, the three p arties are com pressed into tw o; the part one individual p lays is tailored to the parts played by the others present, and yet these others also constitute the audience. Still other inadequacies in th is model will be considered later. The illustrative materials used in this study are of mixed sta tu s: some are taken from respectable rese arch e s where qualified generalisations are given concerning reliably recorded regularities; some are taken from informal memoirs written by colourful peo p le; many fall in between. The justification for th is approach (as I take to be the justification for Simmei’s also) is that the illustrations together fit into a coherent framework that ties together bits of experience the reader h as already had and provides the student with a guide worth testing in casestu d ies’of institutional social life. The framework is presented in logical steps. The introduction is necessarilv abstract and may be skipped. T A B L E OF C O N T EK T S PA G E A ck no w ledg em en ts P re fa c e 'N T R O D U C T IO N ............................................................................... 1 CH AP. i P E R F O R M A N C E S ............................................................ 10 II TEAMS ............................................................................... 47 ill REGIONS AND REGION BEHAVIOUR ... 66 iV D IS C R E P A N T R O L E S ......................................... 87 V COMMUNICATION OUT OF C H A R A C T E R ... 107 VI T H E ARTS OF IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT ... 132 VII CONCLUSION ............................................................ 152 INTRODUCTION When an individual enters the presence of others, they commonly seek to acquire information about him or to bring into play information about him already p o s se sse d . They will be in terested in his general socio-econom ic sta tu s, his conception of self, his attitude toward them, his com petence, his trustw orthiness, etc. Although some of this information seem s to be sought almost a s an end in itself, there are usually quite practic al reasons for acquiring it. Information about the individual helps to define the situation, enabling others to know in advance what he will expect of them and what they may expect of him. Informed in th ese ways, the others will know how best to act in order to call forth a desired resp o n se from him. For th o se present, many sources of information become a c c e ssib le and many carriers (or ' s ig n -v e h ic le s’) become available for conveying th is information. If unacquainted with the individual, observers can glean clu es from his conduct and appearance which allow them to apply their previous experience with individuals roughly sim ilar to the one before them or, more important, to apply untested stereotypes to him. They can also assum e from past experience that only individuals of a particular kind are likely to be found in a given social setting. They c a n rely on what the individual say s about him self or on documentary evidence he provides a s to who and what he is. If ttiey know, or know of, the individual by virtue of experience prior to the interaction, they can rely on assum ptions as to the p ersisten c e and generality of psychological traits as a means of predicting his p resen t and future behaviour. However, during the period in which the individual is in the im m ediate p resen ce of the others, few ev en ts may occur which directly provide the others with the conclusive infonnation they will need if they are to direct wisely their own activity. Many crucial facts lie beyond the time and place of interaction or lie concealed within it. For- example, the ' t r u e ’ or ’ r e a l’ attitudes, beliefs, and em otions of the individual can be as c e rtained only indirectly, through his avow als or through what 1 appears to be involuntary expressive behaviour. Similarly, if the individual offers the others a product or service, they will often find that during the interaction there will be no time and p la ce immediately available for eating the pudding that the proof can be found in. They will be forced to accep t some ev en ts a s conventional or natural signs of something not directly available to the se n ses. In Ichheiser’s te r m s 1, the individual will have to act so that he intentionally or unintentionally expresses himself, and the others will in turn have to be impressed in some way by him. We find, then, that when the individual is in the immediate presence of others, his activity will have a promissory character. T he others are likely to find that they must accept the individual on faith, offering him a just return while he is present before them in exchange for something whose true value will not be estab lish ed until after he has left their presence. (Of course, the others also live by inference in their dealings with the physical world, but it is only in the world of social interaction that the o b je cts about which they make inferences will purposely facilitate and hinder this inferential process.) T he security that they justifiably feel in making inferences about the individual will vary, of course, depending on such factors a s the amount of previous information they p o s s e s s about him, but no amount of such past evidence can entirely obviate the n ec essity of acting on the b asis of inferences. L et us now turn from the others to the point of view of the individual who presents him self before them. He may wish them to think highly of him, or to think that he thinks highly of them, or to perceive how in fact he feels toward them, or to obtain no clear-cut im pression; he may wish to ensure sufficient harmony so that the interaction can be sustained, or to defraud, get rid of, confuse, m islead, antagonize, or insult them. R egardless of the particular objective which the individual has in mind and of h is motive for having th is objective, it will be in his in te rests to control the conduct of the others, especially their responsive treatment of him. 2 T h is control is achieved largely by influencing the definition of the situation which the others come to formulate, and he can influence this definition by expressing himself in such a way as to give them the kind of im pression 'G u s ta v Icheiser, ‘ Misunderstandings in Human R e la tio n s ', Supplement to The A m erican Journal o f Sociology, LV, (September, 1949) pp. 6-7. a i!ere 1 owe much to an unpublished paper by Tom Burns of the University of Edinburgh, where the argument is presented rhat in all interaction a basic underlying theme is the desire of each participant to guide and control the response made by the others present. 2 that will lead them to act voluntarily in accordance with h is own plan. Thus, when an individual appears in the presence of others, there will usually be some reason for him to mobilize his activity so that it will convey an im pression to others which it is in his in te rests to convey. I have said that when an individual appears before others his actions will influence the definition of the situation which they come to have. Sometimes the individual will ac t in a thoroughly calculating manner, expressing him self in a given way solely in order to give the kind of impression to others that is likely to evoke from them a specific response he is concerned to obtain. Sometimes the individual will be calculating in h is activity but be relatively unaware that this is the case. Sometimes he will intentionally and consciously express himself in a particular way, but chiefly because the tradition of his group or social sta tu s require this kind of expression and not b ec au se of any particular response (other than vague acceptance or approval) that is likely to be evoked from those impressed by the expression. Sometimes the traditions of an individual’s role will lead him to give a well-designed im pression of a particular kind and yet he may be neither consciously nor unconsciously disposed to create such an impression. The others, in their turn, may be suitably im pressed by the individual’s efforts to convey something, or may sceptically examine a s p e c ts of his activity of whose significance he is not aware, or may misunderstand the situation and come to conclusions that are warranted neither by the individual’s intent nor by the facts. In any case, in so far as the others act as i f the individual had conveyed a particular impression, we may take a functional or pragmatic view and say that the individual has ’effectiv e ly ’ projected a given definition of the situation and 'e f fe c tiv e ly ’ fostered the understanding that a given sta te of affairs obtains. When we allow that the individual projects a definition of the situation when he appears before others, we must also see chat the others, however p assiv e their role may seem to be, will them selves effectively project a definition of the situation by virtue of their response to the individual and by virtue of any lin es of action they initiate to him. Ordinarily we find that the definitions of the situation projected by the several different participants are sufficiently attuned to one another so that open contradiction will not occur. I do not mean that there will be the kind of consensus that arise s when each individual present candidly ex p resses what he really feels and honestly agrees 3 with the expressed feelings of the others present. T his kind of harmony is an optim istic ideal and in any c a se not necessary for the smooth working of society. Rather, each participant is expected to suppress his immediate heartfelt feelings, conveying a view of the situation which he feels the others will be able to find at le a st temporarily acceptable. The m aintenance of th is surface of agreement, this veneer of con sen su s, is facilitated by each participant concealing his own wants behind statem ents which a s se rt values to which everyone p resen t is likely to give lip-service. Further, there is usually a kind of division of definitional labour. Each participant is allow ed to establish the tentative official ruling regarding m atters which are vital to him bat not imm ediately important to others, e.g., the rationalizations and justifications by which he accounts for h is p ast activ ity ; in exchange for th is courtesy he rem ains silent or non-committal on m atters important to others but not immediately important to him. We have then a kind of interactional modus vivendi. Together the participants contribute to a single overall definition of the situation which involves not so much a real agreement as to what e x ists but rather a real agreement as to whose claim s concerning what is s u e s will be temporarily honoured. Real agreement will also exist concem the desirability of avoiding an open conflict of definitions of the situation. 1 L et us refer to this level of agreement as a 'w orking c o n s e n s u s ’. It is to be understood that th e working co n sen su s established in one interaction setting will be quite different in content from the working co n sen su s esta b lish ed in a different type of setting. T hus, between two friends at lunch, a reciprocal show of affection, resp ect, and concern for the other i s maintained. In service occupations, on the other hand, the sp ecialist often m aintains an image of d isin terested involvement in the problem of the client, while the client responds with a show of respect for the com petence and integrity of the sp e cia list. R egardless of such differences in content, however, the general form of th e se working arrangements is the same. In noting the tendency for a participant to accept the definitional claim s made by the others present, we can appreciate the crucial importance of the -information that the individual initially p o s s e s s e s or acq u ires concerning his fellow partici­ 1An interaction can be purposely set up a s a time and p lace for voicing differences in opinion, but in such c a s e s participants must be careful to agree not to disagree on the proper tone of voicc, vocabulary, and degree of se rio u s n e ss in which all arguments are to be phrased, and upon the mutual resp ect which disagreeing participants must carefully continue to ex press toward one another. T h is d eb aters' or academ ic definition of the situation may also be suddenly and judiciously invoked as a way of tran slating a serious conflict of view s into one that can be handled within a framework accep tab le to all present. 4 pants, for it i s on the b a s is of th is initial information that the individual s ta rts to define the situation and s ta rts to build up lin e s of resp o n siv e action. T he in d ividual’s initial projection com mits him to what he is proposing to be and req u ires him to drop all p rete n ce s of being other things. ■ As th e interaction among the p articip an ts p rogresses, additions and m odifications in th is initial informational s ta te will of co u rse occur, -but it is essen tial that th e se later developm ents be related withouc contradiction to, and even built up from, the initial p ositions taken by the several p articipants. It would seem that an individual can more ea sily make a choice a s to what line of treatment to demand from and extend to the others present at the beginning o f an encounter than he can alter the lin e of treatment th at is being pursued once the interaction i s underway. In everyday life, of course, there is a clea r understanding that first im pressions are important. T hus, the work adjustm ent of th o se in serv ice occupations will often hinge upon a capacity to se iz e and hold the in itiativ e in the service relation, a capac ity that will require subtle a g g re ssiv e n e ss on the part of the server when he is of lower socio-econom ic sta tu s than his client. W. F. Whyte su g g e sts the w aitress as an exam ple: T h e firsc point that sta n d s out i s that the w aitress who bears up under p ressure does not simply respond ro her custom ers. She a c ts with som e skill to control their behaviour. ] The first question to ask when we look at the custom er relatio nsh ip is, “ D oes the w a itre ss get the jump on the customer, or does the custom er get the jump on the w a i t r e s s ? '1 T he skilled w aitress r e a liz e s the crucial nature of th is question . . . . The sk illed w a itre ss ta c k le s the custom er with confidence and without h esita tio n . For exam ple, she may find that a new custom er h as se ated him self before she could clear off the dirty d ish e s and change rhe cloth. He i s now leaning on the table studying the menu. She greets him, sa y s, “ May I change the cover, p le a s e and, without waiting for an answ er, takes his menu away from him so that he moves back from the table, and sh e goes about her work. T h e relatio n sh ip ,is handled politely but firmly, and there is never any question a s to who is in charge. 1 When the interaction that is initiated by " f i r s t im p re ssio n s” i s its e lf merely th e initial interaction in an extended s e rie s of in te ra ctio n s involving the sam e p articip an ts, we speak of ''getting off on the right fo o t” and feel that it is crucial that we do so. T hus, one le arn s that som e te ac h ers take the following v ie w : You c a n 't ever ler them get the upper hand on you or you're through. So I sta rt out tough. T he first day I get a new c l a s s in, I let them know who’s b o ss . . . . You've got to sta rt oil tough, then you can ea s e up a s you go along. If you sta rt out easy-going, when you try to get tough, th ey ’ll just look at you and l a u g h . 2 1 W. F. Whyte, 11 When Workers and C ustom ers M eet,” Chap. VII, Industry am i S o ciety, e d .W .F . Whyte (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1946), pp. 132-133? T e a c h e r interview quoted by Howard S. Becker, " S o c ia l C l a s s V ariations in rhe T eacher-P upil R e la tio n s h ip ," Journal o f E ducational Sociology, XXV, 459. 5 Similarly, attendants ih mental institutions may feel that if the new patient i s sharply put in his p lace the first day on the ward and made to se e who i s boss, much future difficulty will be prevented. 1 Given the fact that the individual effectively p ro jects a definition of the situation when he enters the presence of others, we can assum e that events may occur within the interaction which contradict, discredit, or otherw ise throw doubt upon th is projection. When th ese disruptive events occur, the interaction its elf may come to a confused and em barrassed halt. Some of the assum ptions upon which the responses of the particip an ts had been predicated become untenable, and the particip an ts find them selves lodged in an interaction for which the situation h as been wrongly defined and is now no longer defined. At such moments the individual whose presentation h as been discredited may feel asham ed while the others present may feel hostile, and all the participants may come to feel ill at ease, nonplussed, our of countenance, em barrassed, experiencing the kind of anomie that is generated when the minute social system of face-to-face interaction breaks down. In stressin g the fact that the initial definition of the situation projected by an individual tends to provide a plan for the co-operative activity that follow s—in stressin g this action point o f view —we must not overlook the crucial fact that any projected definition of the situation also h as a distinctive moral character. It i s this moral character of projections that will chiefly concern us in this report. Society is organized on the principle that any individual who p o s s e s s e s certain social characteristics has a moral right to expect that others will value and treat him in a correspondingly appropriate way. Connected with this principle is a second, namely that an individual who implicitly.or explicitly signifies that he has certain social ch aracteristics ought to have this claim honoured by others and ought in fact to be what he claim s he is. In consequence, when an individual projects a definition of the situation and thereby makes an implicit or explicit claim to be a person of a particular kind, he autom atically exerts a moral demand upon the others, obliging them to value and treat him in the manner that persons of his kind have a right to expect. He also im plicitly forgoes all claim s to be things he does not appear to b e 2 and hence forgoes the treatment that would be 1 Harold T axel, 'Authority Structure in a Mental Hospital Ward’, Unpublished Master’s th e s is , Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1953- 2 T hia role of the w itness in limiting what it is the individual can be has been stressed by E xistentialists, who see ic as a basic threat ro individual freedom. See Jean-P aul Sartre; L ’etre e t le neant ( P a ris : Gallimard, 1948), p. 319 ff. 6 appropriate for such individuals TKv. others find, then, chit the individual has informed them as to whac is and a s to what ^hey ought to s e e as the ' i s ’. We cannot judge the importance of definitional disruptions by the fre<|'iency with which they occur, for apparently they n-rmld occur more frequently were noc constant precautions taken. We find that preventive p ractices are constantly employed to u.void th e se em barrassm ents and that corrective practic e s are constantly employed to com pensate for discrediting o ccurrences that have not been su ccessfu lly avoided. When the individual employs th ese strateg ie s and ta c tic s to protect his own projections, we may refer to them a s 'd e fe n s iv e pract i c e s ’ ; when a participant employs them to save the definition of the situation projected by another, we speak of 'p ro te ctiv e p ra c tic e s ’ or ' t a c t ’. Together, ’defensive and protective practic e s comprise the techniques employed to safeguard the impression fostered by an individual during his p resen ce before others. It should be added that while we are perhaps ready to see that no fostered im pression would survive if defensive p ractices were not employed, we are perhaps le s s ready to se e that few im pressions could survive if those who received the im pression did not exert tact in their reception of it. In addition to the fact that precautions are taken to prevent disruption of projected definitions, we may also note that an intense interest in th ese disruptions comes to play a significant role in the social life of the group. P ractical jokes and social games are played in which em barrassm ents which are to be taken unseriously are purposely engineered. 1 P h an ta sie s are created in which devastating exposures occur. A necdotes from the p a s t—real, embroidered, or fictitious—are told and retold, detailing disruptions which occurred, almost occurred, or occurred and were admirably resolved. There seem s to be no grouping which does not have a ready supply of th e se games, reveries, and cautionary tales, to be used a s a source of humour, a c a th a rsis for anxieties, and a sanction for inducing individuals to be modest in their claim s and reasonable in their projected expectations. T he individual m ay tell him self through dreams of getting into im possible positions. Fam ilies tell of the time a guest got his d ates mixed and arrived when neither the house nor anyone in it was ready for him. Jo u rn alists tell of tim es when an all-too-meaningful misprint occurred, and the paper’s assumption of objectivity or decorum was humorously discredited. Public Servants tell of times a client ridiculously 1Erving Goffmin, 'C ommunication Conduct in an Island Com m unity’ (Unpublished Ph.D . dissertatio n, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1953), pp. 319-327. 7 mis-understood form instructions, giving answ ers which implied ;m unanticipated and bizarre definition of the situation. 1 Seamen, whose home away from home is rigorously he-man, tell stories of coming back home and inadvertently asking mother to " p a s s the f-cking b u tte r” . 2 Diplomats tell of the time a nearsighted Queen asked a republican am bassador about the health of h is King. 3 To summarize, then, I assum e that when an individual appears before others he will have many motives for trying to control the im pression they receive of the situation. T h is report is concerned with some of the common techniques that in teractan ts employ to sustain such im pressions and with some of the common contingencies asso ciate d with the employment of th ese techniques. The specific content of any activity presented by the individual participant, or the role it pl.\ys in the interdependent a c tiv itie s of an on-going social system , will not be at is s u e ; I shall be concerned only with the p articip an t’s dramaturgical problems of presenting the activity before others. The iss u e s dealt with by stage-craft and stage-management are sometimes trivial but they are quite general; they seem to occur everywhere in social life, providing a clear-cut dimension for formal sociological analysis. It will be convenient to end th is introduction with some definitions that are implied in whac has gone before an.) required for what is to follow. For the purpose of this report, interaction (that .is, face-to-face interaction) may be roughly •Jctined as the reciprocal influence of individuals upon one another’s actions when in one another’s immediate physical presence. An interaction may be defined as all the interaction which occurs throughout any one occasion when a given set of individuals are in one another’s continuous presence; the term 'a n encounter’ would do as well. A ‘ perform ance’ may be defined a s all the activity of a given participant on a given occasion which serves to influence in any way any of the other participants. Taking a particular participant and his performance a s a b asic point of reference, we may refer ro those who contribute the other perform ances as the audience, observers, or co-participants. The pre-established pattern of action which is unfolded during a performance and which may be presented or 1Peter lilau, ' Dynamics of Bureaucracy ' (Ph.D. dissertatio n. Department of Sociology, Columbia University, forthcoming, University of Chicago P tcss), pp. 127-129. 2 ^’nlter M. b eattie, Jr., 'T h e Merchant S eam an' (Unpublished M.A. Report, Department of Sociology, University of Cliic.iRo, 1950), |>. 35. 3 Sir l-'redcrick Ponsonby, R eco llectio n s o f Three R eien s (New York: Dutton, 1952), p. 46. 8 played through on other o cc asio n s may be called a 'p a r t ’ or ‘ ro u tin e’. 1 T h ese situational terms can easily be related to conventional structural ones. When an individual or performer p lays the sam e part to the sam e audience on different o c c asions, -a social relationship is likely to arise. Defining social role as the enactment of rights and d u ties attached to a given status, 'We can say that a social role will involve one or more parts and that each of th ese different parts may be presented by the performer on a se ries of o ccasions to the sam e kinds of audience or to an audience of the sam e persons. 1For comments on the importance of distinguishing between a routine of interaction and any particular instance when this routine is played through, s e e John von Neumann and Oskar Morgenstern, The Theory o f Games ana E conom ic B ehaviour (2nd e d . ; P.-inceton: Princeton University P ress, 1947), p. 49. C H A P T E R I PERFORMANCES Belief in the Part One is Playing When an individual plays a part he implicitly requests his observers to take seriously the impression that is fostered before them. They are asked to believe that the character they s e e actually p o s s e s s e s the attributes he appears to p o sse ss, that the task he performs will have the consequences that are implicitly claim ed for it, and that, in general, m atters are what they appear to be. In line with this, there is the popular view that the individual offers his performance and p uts on his show ' for the benefit of other people.' It will be convenient to begin a consideration of performances by turning the question around and looking at the individual’s own belief in the impression of reality that he attem pts to engender in those among whom he finds himself. At one extreme, ■we find that the performer can be fully taken in by his own a c t; he can be sincerely convinced that the impression of reality which he sta g es is the real reality. When his audience is also convinced in this way about the show he puts o n —and th is seem s to be the typical c a se —then for the moment, anyway, only the sociologist or the socially disgruntled will have any doubts about the 'r e a l n e s s ’ of what is presented. At the other extreme, we find that the performer may not be taken in at all by his own routine. This possibility is understa n d ab le, since no one is in quite a s good an observational position to se e through the act a s the person who puts it on. Coupled with this, the performer may be moved to .guide the conviction of his audience only a s a m eans to other ends, having no ultimate concern in the conception that they have of him or of the situation. When the individual has no belief in h is own a c t and no ultimate concern with the beliefs of his audience, we may call him cynical, reserving the term sincere for individuals who believe in the impression fostered by their own performance. It should be understood that the cynic, with all his professional disinvolvem ent, may obtain unprofessional pleasures from his masquerade, experiencing a kind of gleeful spiritual aggression from the fact that he can toy at will with something his audience must take se rio u s ly .1 ’ P erhaps the real crime of the confidence man is not that he ta k e s money from his victims buc chat he robs all of us of the belief chat m iddle-class manners and appearance can be sustained only by m iddle-class people. A 10 It is not assumed, of course, that all cynical performers are interested in deluding their audiences for purposes of what is called 's e lf - in te re s t' or private gain. A cynical individual may delude h is audience for what he considers to be their own good, or for the good of the community, etc. For illustrations of this we need not appeal to sadly enlightened showmen such as Marcus Aurelius or Hsun Tzu. We know that in service occupations practitioners who may otherwise be sincere are sometim es forced to delude their custom ers because their custom ers show such a heartfelt demand for it. Doctors who are led into giving placebos, filling-station attendants who resignedly check and recheck tire p ressures for anxious women motorists, shoe clerks who sell a shoe that fits but tell the customer it is the size she w ants to hear—these are cynical performers whose audiences will not allow them to be sincere. Similarly, we find that sym pathetic p atients in mental wards will som etim es feign bizarre symptoms so that student nurses will not be subjected to a disappointingly sane performance. 1 So also, when inferiors extend their most lavish reception for visiting superiors, the selfish desire to win favour may not be the chief motive; the inferior may be tactfully attempting to put the superior at ea se by sim ulating the kind of world the superior is thought to take for granted. I have suggested two extrem es: an individual may be taken in by his own act or be cynical about it. T hese extremes are something a little more than just the ends of a continuum. Each provides the individual with a position which h as its own particular secu rities and defences, so there will be a tendency for those who have travelled close to one of th ese poles to complete the voyage. Starting with lack of inward belief in one’s role, the individual may follow the natural movement described by P a r k : It is probably no mere historical accident that the word person, in its first meaning, ts a mask. It is rather a recognition of the fact that disabused professional can be cynically hostile 10 the service relation his clien ts expect him to extend to them ; the confidence man is in a position to hold the whole 1legit * world in this contempt. 'S e e L'dxel, op. c it., p. 4. Marry Stack Sullivan has suggested that the tact of institutionalized performers can operate in the ocher direction, resulting in a kind of noblessc^oblige sanity. bee his ‘ Socio-Psychiatric Research Am erican Journal o f P sychiatry, X, pp. 987*988. MA study of ’.social recoveries 1 in one of our large mental hospitals some y ears ago caught me that patients were often released from care because they had learned noc to manifest symptoms to the environing p erso n s; in other words, had integrated enough of the personal environment to realize the prejudice opposed to their delusions. It seem ed almost as if they grew wise enough to be tolerant of the tmbeciliry surrounding them, having finally discovered that it was stupidity and not malice. They could then secure satisfaction from co n tact with others, while discharging a part of their cravings by psychotic means. 11 everyone is always and everywhere, more or le s s consciou sly , playing a role . . . It i s in these ro les that we know each o th er; it is in th ese ro le s that we know ourselv es. 1 In a sense, and in so Jar as th is mask represents the conception we have formed of ourselves—the role we are striving to live up to —tliis m ask is our truer se lf, the se lf we would like to be. In th e end, our conception of our role becom es second nature and an integral part of our personality. come into the world as individuals, achieve character, and become p e rso n s .2 T h is may be illuscrated from the writer’s study of an island community of crofters, that is, sm all-holding farmers. 3 For the la st four or five y ears the is la n d 's tourist hotel has been owned and operated by a married couple of crofter origins. From the beginning, the owners were forced to set aside their own conceptions a s to how life ought to be led, displaying in the hotel a full round of m iddle-class se rv ices and am enities. Lately, however, it appears that the managers have become le s s cynical about the performance that they s ta g e ; they them selves are becoming middle c la s s and more and more enamoured of the se lv es their c lie n ts impuce to them. Another illustration may be found in the raw recruit who initially follows army etiquette in order to avoid physical punishment and who eventually comes to follow the rules so that his organization will not be shamed and his officers and fellow -soldiers will respect him. As suggested, the cycle of disbelief-to-belief can be followed in the other direction, startin g with conviction or insecure aspiration and ending in cynicism . Professions which the public holds in religious awe often allow their recruits to follow the cycle in th is direction, anti often recruits follow it in this direction not because of a slow realization that they are deluding their au d ien ce—for by ordinary social standards the claim s they make may be quite valid—but because they can use this cynicism a s a means of insulating their inner s e lv e s from contact with the audience. ■ And we may even expect to find typical careers of faith, with the individual starting out with one kind of involvement in the performance he is required to give, then moving back and forth several tim es between sin c erity and cynicism before com pleting all the p h ases and turningpoints of self-belief for a person of his station. While we can expect to find natural movement back and forth between cynicism and sincerity, s till we must not rule out the kind of transitional points that can be sustained, on the l Robert Ezra Park, R a ce and Culture (G lencoe.|lll.: T h e Free P re s s , 1950), p. 249. 3 H id ., p. 250. 3 The study w as financed by the Department of Social Anthropology And th e Social S cien ces R esearch Committee of the University of Kdinburgh and reported io part in Goffman, op. c it. 12 strength of a little self-illusion. We find that the individual may attempt to induce the audience to judge him and the situation in a particular way, and he may seek th is judgement as an ultimate end in itself, and yet he may not com pletely believe that he deserves the valuation of self which he ask s for or that the impression of reality which he fosters is valid. Another mixture of cynicism and belief is suggested in Kroeber’s d iscussion of sham anism : Next, there is the old question of deception« Probably m ost shamans or medicine men, the world over, help along with sleight-of-hand in curing and especially in exhibitions of power. T his sleight-of-hand is sometimes d elib erate; in many c a s e s aw areness is perhaps not deeper chan the foreconscious. The attitude, whether there has been repression or not, seem s to be a s toward a pious fraud. Field ethnographers seem quite generally convinced that even sham ans who know that they add fraud neverth eless also believe in their powers, and especially in those of other sh am an s: they consult them whet! they them selves or theLr children are ill. 1 Front We have been using the term 'perform ance' to refer to all the activity of an individual which occurs during a period marked by his continuous presence before a particular set of observers and which has some influence on the observers. It will be convenient to label as 'f r o n t ’ that part of the individual’s performance which regularly functions in a general and fixed fashion to define the situation for those who observe the performance. Front, then, is the expressive equipment of a standard kind intentionally or unwittingly employed by the individual during his performance. For preliminary purposes, it v.ill be convenient to distinguish and label what seem to be the stand aril parts of front. First, there is the 's e t t i n g ’, involving furniture, decor, physical lay-out, and other background item s which supply the scenery and stage props for the sp ate of human action played out before, within, or upon it. A setting tends to stay put, geographically speaking, so that those who would use a particular setting a s part of their performance cannot begin their act until they have brought them selves to the appropriate place and must term inate their performance when they leave it. It is only in exceptional circum stances that the setting, in a sense, follows along with the perform ers; we see this in the funeral cortege, the civic parade, and the dream-like p ro cessio n s that" kings and queens are made of. In the main, th e se exceptions seem to offer some kind of extra protection for performers who ; A.I.. Krocber, The feature o f Culture (C hicago: University of Chicago P ress, ll)^2), p. 3 N . 13 are, or who have momentarily become, highly sacred. I hese worthies are to be distinguished, of course, from quite profane performers of the peddler c la s s who move their pl;ice of work between performances, often being forced to do so. In the matter of having one fixed place for o n e 's setting, a ruler may be too sacred, a peddler too profane. In thinking about the scenic a s p e c ts of front, we tend to think of the living room in a particular house and the small number of performers who can thoroughly identify them selves with it. We have given insufficient attention to assem blages of of sign-equipment which large numbers of performers can call their own for short periods of time. It is characteristic of Western European countries, and no doubt a source of stability f o r ^ , 1" that a large number of luxurious settings are available for hire to anyone of the right kind who can afford them. One illustration of this may be cited from a study of the higher civil servant in Britain: T he question liow far the men who rise to the top in the Civil Service take on the 'tone* or 'colour* of a c la s s other than that to which they belong by birth is delicate and difficult. The only definite information bearing on the question is the figures relating to the membership of the great London clubs. More than three-quarters of our high adm inistrative o fficials belong to one or more clubs of high sta tu s and considerable Luxury, where the entrance fee might be twenty guineas or more, and rhe annual subscription from twelve to twenty guineas. T h ese in stitu tio ns are of the upper c la s s (not even of the upper-middle) in their prem ises, their equipment, the style of living practised there, their whole atmosphere. Though many of the members would not be described a s wealthy, only a wealthy man would unaided provide for himself and his family sp ace, food and drink, service, and other am enities of life to the same standard a s he will find at the Uruon, the T rav ellers’, or the Reform. 1 Another example can be found in the recent development of the medical profession where we find that it is increasingly important for a doctor to have a c c e s s to the elaborate scientific stage provided by large hospitals, so that fewer and fewer doctors are able to feel that their settin g is a place that they can lock up at night. 2 If we take the term 's e t t i n g ’ to refer to the scenic parts ol expressive equipment, -we may take the term 'p erso n al front’ to refer to the other items of expressive equipment, the item s that we most intimately identify with the performer himself and that we naturally expect will follow the performer wherever he goes. As part of personal front we may include: insignia of office or rank; clothing; sex, age, and racial ch arac teristics; size and lo o k s; p o sture; speech pattern s; facial ex p ressio n s; bodily 1U.K . Dale, T ke Higher C ivil Service o f Great ftritain (Oxford: Oxford University P re ss , 1941), p. 50. 2 David Solomon, 'C a re e r Contingencies ol Chicago P h y s ic ia n s ' (Unpublished Ph. IX dissertation, Department ol Sociology, University of Chicaao. 1952). p. 74. 14 g estu re s; an 1 the like. Some of these vehicles for conveying signs, such a s racial characteristics, are relatively fixed and over a span of time Jo not vary for the individual from one situation to another. On the orhcr hand, some of these sign vehicles are relatively mobile or transitory, such as facial exp-ession, an 1 can vary .luring a performance from one moment to the next. It is sometimes convenient to divide the stimuli which make up personal front into 'a p p e a ra n c e ’ and ’ manner’, according to the function performed by the information that these stimuli convey. ' A ppearance’ may be taken to refer to those stimuli which function at the time to tell us of the performer’s social statu ses. T hese stimuli also tell us of the individual's temporary ritual state, that is, whether he is engaging in formal social activity, work, or informal recreation, whether or not he is celebrating a new phase in the season cycle or in his life-cycle. 'M anner' may be taken to refer to those stimuli which function at the time to warn u s of the interaction role the performer will expect to play in the on-coming situation. T hus a haughty aggressive manner may give the impression that the performer expects to be the one who will in itiate the verbal interaction and direct its course. A meek, apologetic manner may give the impression that the performer expects to follow the lead of others, or at le a st that he can 1>e gotten to do so. Similarly, if an individual is angry his manner will tell us upon whom he is likely to be in a position to vent his anger. We often expect, of course, a confirming consistency between appearance and manner; we expect that the differences in social sta tu s e s among the ioteractants will be expressed in some way by congruent differences in the indications that are made of expected interaction role. T his type of coherence of front may be illustrated by the following description of the procession of a mandarin through a Chinese city: Coming closely behind . . . the luxurious chair of the mandarin, carried by eight bearers, fills the vacant space in the street. Me is mayor of the town, and for all practical purposes die supreme power in it. lie is an ideal-looking official, for he is large and m assive in appearance, whilst he has that stern and uncomprising look that is supposed to be n ecessary in any magistrate who would hope to keep his sub jects in order, lie lias a stem and forbidding asp ect, as though he were on his way to the execution ground to have some criminal decapitated. T his is the kind of air that the mandarins put on when they appear in public. In the course of many years' experience. 1 have never once seen any of them, from the highest to the lowest, with a smile on his facc or a look of sympathy for the people whilst he was being carricd officially through the streets. 1 b u t, of course, appearance and manner may tend to contradict each other, as when a performer who appears to be of higher 1J . Macgowart, S id elig h ts on C hinese L ife (Philadelp hia: Lippincoct, 1908), p. 187. 15 e s ta te than h is audience a c ts in a manner that is unexpectedly equalitarian, or intimate, or apologetic, or when a performer dressed in the garments of a high position presents himself to an individual of even higher sta tu s. In addition to the expected co n sisten cy between appearance and manner, we expect, of course, some coherence among setting, appearance, and manner. 1 In a sen se, such coherence represents an ideal type that provides us with a means of stimulating our interest in and attention to exceptions. In this the student is assisted by the journalist, for exceptions to expected co n sisten cy among setting, appearance, .and manner provide the piquancy and glamour of many careers and the saleable appeal of many magazine articles. 2 In order to explore more fully the relations among the several parts of social front, it will be convenient to consider here a significant characteristic of the information conveyed by front, namely, its a b stra c tn ess and generality. However specialized and unique a routine is, its social front, with certain exceptions, will tend to claim facts that can be equally claimed and asserted of other, somewhat different routines. ; For example, many service occupations offer their clien ts a performance that is illuminated with dramatic expressions of clean lin ess, modernity, com petence, integrity, etc. While in fact th ese abstract standards have a different significance in different occupational performances, the observer is encouraged to s tre s s the abstract sim ilarities. For the observer this is a wonderful, chough som etim es disastrous, convenience. Instead of having to maintain a different pattern of expectation and responsive treatment for each slightly different performer and performance, he can place the situation into a broad category around which it is easy for him to mobilize his past experience and stereo-typical thinking. O bservers then need only be familiar with a small and hence manageable vocabulary of fronts and know how to respond to them in order to orient themse lv es in a wide variety of situations. T h u s in London the current tendency for chimney s w e e p s 3 and perfume clerks to wear white lab coats tends to provide the client with an understanding that the delicate ta s k s performed by th ese persons 1Cl. Kenneth B urke's comments on the ' scene-act-agent r a t i o ’, A Grammar o f M otives (New York: P rentice-H all, 1945) pp. 6-9. ^ For example, the New Yorker Profile on Roger Stevens (the real e s ta te agent who engineered the s a le of the Empire State Building) comments on the startling fact that Stevens h as a small house, a meagre office, and no letterhead stationery. See E. J.K ah n , Jr., 'C lo s in g s and O pen ing s', The New Yorker, February 13 and 20, 1954. 3 S ec Mervyn Jo n e s, 'White a s a S w eep ', The N ew Statesm an and Nation, December 6, 1952. 16 will be performed in what has become a standardized, clinical, confidential manner. T here are grounds for believing that the tendency for a large number of different a c ts to be presented from behind a small number of fronts is a natural development in social organization. Kadcliffe-Hrown has suggested th is in his claim that a descriptive kinship system which gives each person a unique place may work for very small communities, but, a s the number of persons becom es large, clan segmentation becomes n ecessary a s a means of providing a le ss complicated system of identifications and treatm ents. 1 We see this tendency illustrated in factories, barracks, and other large social estab lish ments. Those who organize these establishm ents find it impossible to provide a special cafeteria, special modes of payment, special vacation rights, and special sanitary facilities for evc-ry line and staff sta tu s category in the organization, and at the same time they feel that persons of dissim ilar sta tu s ought not to be indiscrim inately thrown together or classified together. As a compromise, the full range of diversity is cut at a few crucial points, and all those within a given bracket are allowed or obliged to maintain the same social front in certain situations. In addition to the fact that different routines may employ the same front, it is to be noted that a given social front tends to become institutionalized in terms of the abstract stereotyped expectations to which it gives rise, and tends to take on a meaning and stability apart from the specific ta s k s which happen at the time to be performed in its name. T he front becomes a 'c o lle c tiv e representation’ and a fact in its own right. When an actor tak es on an established social role, usually he finds that a particular front has already been estab lish ed for it. Whether his acquisition of the role was primarily motivated by a desire to perform the given task or by a desire to maintain the corresponding front, the actor will find that he must do both. Further, if the individual tak es on a task that is not only new to him but also unestablished in the so c iety .-o r if he attem pts to change the light in which his task is viewed, he is likelv to find that there are already several w ell-established fronts among which he must choose. Thus, when a task is given a new front we seldom find that the front it is given is itself new. Since fronts tend ro be selected, not created, we may expect trouble to arise when those who perform a given task are forced 1 A. U. Radcliffe-ftro wn, ’ The Social Organization of Australian T r i b e s ’, O ceania, 1, 440. 17 to se le c t a su itab le front for them selves from among several quite dissim ilar ones. Thus, in military organizations, ta s k s are alw ays developing which (it is felt) require too much authority and skill to be carried out behind the front maintained by one grade of personnel and too little authority an.) sk i’l to be carried out behind the front maintained by the ne-'r ;ra.l<* in the hierarchy. Since there are relatively large jurm s between grades, the rask will come to 'carry too much . a r k 1 or to carry too little. An interesting illustration of the dilemma of selectin g an appropriate front from sev eral not quite fitting oi'es may be found today in American medical organizations with respect to the ta s k of adm inistering a n e s t h e s i a . 1 In so m : h o spitals a n e sth e sia i s still adm inistered by nu rses behind the front that nurses are allowed to have in h o spitals reg ard le ss of the ta sk s they perform—a front involving ceremonial subordination and a relatively low rate of pay. In order to estab lish anesthesiology a s a speciality for graduate medical doctors, interested practitioners have had to advocate strongly the idea that adm inistering an e sth esia is a sufficiently complex and vital task to justifying giving to those who perform it the ceremonial and financial reward given to doctors. T he difference between the front maintained by a nurse and the front maintained by a doctor is great; many things that are acceptable for nu rses are infra dignitatem for doctors. Some medical people have felt that a nurse 'u n d er-ran k ed ' for the task of adm inistering a n e sth e sia and that doctors 'o v e r-ran k e d ;’ were there an estab lish ed sta tu s midway between nurse and doctor, an ea sie r solution to the problem could perhaps be found. 2 Similady, had the Canadian Army had a rank halfway between lieutenant and captain, two and a half pips instead of two or three, then Dental Corp’s captains, many of them of a tow ethnic origin, could have been given a rank that would perhaps have been more suitable in the ey es of the Army than the cap tain cies they were actually given. I do not mean here to s tre s s the point of view of a formal organization or a so c ie ty ; the individual, as someone who p o s s e s s e s a lim ited range of sign-equipment, must also make unhappy choices. Thus, >in the crofting community studied by J S ee the thorough treatment of this problem io Dan C. Lortie, ‘ Doctors Without P a tie n t* : The A nesthesiologist, a New Medical Specialty Unpublished Master’s th esis, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1950. See also Mark Murphv*s tliree-part Profile of Dr. Rovenstine, 1 A n e s t h e s i o l o g i s t The New Yorker. October 25, November 1, and November 8, 1947. ? lr should be added that in some h o sp itals the intern and the m edical student perform task s that are beneath a doctor and above a nurse. Presumably such ta s k s do not require a large amount of experience and practical training» for while this intermediate statu s of doctor-in-training »is a perm anent pare of hospitals, all tho se who hold it do so temporarily. 18 the writer, 'hosts often marked the visit of a friend by offering him a dram of spirits, a glass of wine, some (home-made) brew, o r a cup of tea. The higher the rank or temporary ceremonial sta tu s of the visitor, the more likely he was to receive an offering near the sp irits end of the continuum. Now one problem asso ciated with this range of sign-equipment was that some crofters could not afford to keep a bottle of spirits available, so that wine tended to be the most indulgent gesture they could employ. But perhaps a more common difficulty was the fact that certain visitors, given their permanent and temporary statu s at the time, outranked one potable and under-ranked the next one in line. There was often a danger that the v isito r would feel just a little affronted or, on the other hand, that the host’s costly and limited sign equipment would be m isused. In our middle c la s s e s a similar situation a rise s when a h o ste ss has to decide whether or not to use the good silver, or which would be the more appropriate to wear, her best afternoon dress or her plainest evening gown. [ have suggested that social front can be divided into traditional parts, such a s setting, appearance, and manner, and that (since different routines may be presented from behind the same front) we may not find a perfect fit between the specific character of a performance and the general so cialized guise in which it appears to us. T hese two facts, taken together, lead u s to appreciate that item s in the social front of a particular routine are not only found in the social fronts of a whole range of routines but also that the whole range of routines in which one item of sign equipment is found will differ from the range of routines in which another item in the sam e so cial front will be found. Thus, a lawyer may talk to a client in a social setting that he employs only for this purpose (or for a study), but the clothes he w ears on such occasions, 'and which are suitable for such occasions, he will also employ, with equal suitability, .at dinner with colleagues and at the theatre with his wife. Similarly, the prints that hang on his wall and the carpet on his floor may be found in domestic social establishm ents. Of course, in highly ceremonial occasions, setting, manner, and appearance may all be relatively unique and specific, used only for performances of a single type of routine, but such exclusive use of sign equipment i s the exception rather than the rule. Dramatic Realization While in the presence of others, the individual typically infuses his activity with signs which dramatically highlight and portray confirmatory facts that might otherwise remain unapparent or obscure. For if the individual’s activity is to 19 become significant to others, 'he must m obilize his activity so that it will ex p ress during the interaction what he w ish es to convey. In fact, th e performer may be required not only to ex p ress his claim ed ca p ac itie s during the interaction but also to do so during a split second in the interaction. T h u s, if a baseball umpire is to give the impression th a t he is sure of his judgement, he must forgo the moment of thought which might make him sure of his judgement; he must give an instantaneous decision so that the audience will be sure that he is sure of his judgement. 1 It may be noted that in the case of some s ta tu s e s dramatization presents no problem, since some of the a c ts which are instrum entally e s se n tia l for the completion of the core task of the sta tu s are at the same time wonderfully adapted, from the point of view of communication, as m eans of vividly conveying thé qualities and attributes claimed by the performer. The roles of prizefighters, surgeons, violinists, and policemen are c a s e s in point. T h ese a c tiv itie s allow for so much dramatic self-expression that exemplary practioners—whether real or fictional—become famous and are given a special place in the commercially organized p h an tasies of the nation. In many ca se s, however, dram atization of one's work does constitute a problem. An illustration of this may be cited from a recent study by Edith L entz, where the medical nursing staff in a hospital is shown to have a problem that the surgical nursing staff does not have: T h e things which a nurse does for post-operative patients on the surgical floor are frequently of reco^nizabl e importance, even to patients who are strangers to hospital activ ities. For example, the parient se e s h is nurse changing bandages, swinging orthopedic frames into place, and can realise that these are purposeful activities. Even if she cannot be at h is side, he can re sp e c t h er purposeful activities. Medical nursing is also highly sk illed work . . . . The p h y sic ia n 's diagnosis must rest upon careful observation of symptons over time where the surgeon's are in larger part dependent on visib le things. The lack of visibility creates problems on the m edical. A patient will sec his nurse stop at the next bed and chat for a moment or two with the patient there. He d o esn ’t know chae sh e i s observing the sh allo w ness of the breathing and color and tone of the skin. He thinks she is just visiting. So, alas, does h is family who may thereupon d ecide that these nu rses a ren 't very im pressive. If the nurse spend s more time «u the next bed chan at h is own, the patient may feel slighted . . . • The nurses are “ w asting rim e0 u n le s s they are darting about doing some visib le thing such a s administering hypoderm ics.2 Similarly, we find chat.the proprietor of a serv ice establishm ent may find it difficult to dramatize what is actually being done *See Babe Pinellt, a s told to Joe King. Mr Ump (Ph ilad elp h ia: Westminster P re ss , 1953), p . 75. 2 Edith Lenc2 " A Comparison of Medical and Surgical F lo o rs* 1 (Mimeo: New York Stare School of Industrial and Labour Relations, Cornell University, 1954), pp. 2-3. for clien ts because the clien ts cannot " s e e ” the overhead c o s ts of the service rendered them. T hus trustworthy undertakers must charge a great deal for their highly visible product— a coffin that has been transformed into a c a sk e t—because many of the other co sts of conducting a funeral, are not ones that can be readily dram atized.1 Merchants, too, often find that they must charge high prices for things that look intrinsically expensive in order to com pensate the establishm ent for expensive things like insurance, slack periods, etc., that never appear before the customers’ eyes. The problem of dramatizing o n e 's work involves more than merely making invisible c o s ts visible. The work that must be done by those who fill certain sta tu se s is often so poorly designed as an expression of a desired meaning, that if the incumbent would dramatize the character of his role, 'he must divert an appreciable amount of his energy to do so. And th is activity diverted to communication will often require different attributes from the ones which are being dramatized. T hus to furnish a house so that it will express simple, quiet dignity, the householder may have to race to auction sales, haggle with antique dealers, and doggedly ca n va ss all the local shops for proper wallpaper and curtain materials. To give a radio talk that will sound genuinely informal, spontaneous, and relaxed, the speaker may have to design his script with painstaking care, testing one phrase after another, in order to follow the content, language, rhythm, and pace of everyday talk. 2 Similarly, a Vogue model, by her clothing, stance, and facial expression, is able expressively to portray a cultivated understanding of the book she poses in her hand; but those who trouble to express them selves so appropriately will have very little time left over'for reading. And so individuals often find them selves with the dilemma of expression versus action. Those who have the time and talent to perform a task well may not, because of this, have the time or talent to make it apparenc that they are performing well. It may be said that some organizations resolve this dilemma for these members by delegating the dramatic function to a specialist who will spend his time expressing the meaning of the task and spend no time actually doing it. If we alter our frame of reference for a moment and turn from a particular performance to the individuals who present it, 'M aterial on the burial business used 'hronihout tm s report is taken from a forthcoming dissertation on the funetal director by Robert Habenstein. I have also drawn on Mr Hafcenstein's seminar report describing the undertaker’s work a s the staging of a performance. 2 John Hilton, " C a lc u la te d Sponcaniety,” Oxford B ook o f E nglish Talk fOxford: Clarendon P ress, 1953), pp. 399-404. 21 we can consider an interesting fact about the round of different routines which any group or c l a s s of individuals h elps to perform. When we examine a group or cla ss, we find that the members of it tend to invest their egos primarily in certain routines, giving le s s s tre s s to the other o nes which they perform. T h u s a professional man may be willing to take a very modest role in the street, in a shop, or in h is home, but, in the social sphere which encom passes h is display of professional competency, he will be much concerned to make an effective showing. In mobilizing his behaviour to make a showing, he will be concerned not so much with the full round of th e different routines he performs but only with the one from which h is occupational reputation derives. It is upon th is is s u e that some writers have chosen to distinguish groups with aristocratic •habits (whatever their social status) from those of m iddle-class character. T he aristocratic habit, •it h a s been said, is one that m obilizes all the minor ac tiv ities of life which fall outside the serious sp e cia lities of other c la s s e s and in je cts into th ese a c tiv itie s an expression of character, power, and high rank. By what Important accomplishments is the young nobleman instructed to support the dignity of h is rank, and to render himself worthy of that superiority over h is fellow-citi zens, to which the virtue of h is ancestors had raised them ? Is it by knowledge, by industry, by patience, by self-denial, or by virtue of any kind? As all his words, a s a l l his motions are attended to, he learns an habitual regard to every circumsta n ce of ordinary behaviour, and studies to perform all those small d u ties with the most exact propriety. As he is conscious ol how much he is observed, and how much mankind arc disposed to favour ali h is inclinations, he acts, upon the most indifferent o ccasio n s, with that, freedom and elevation which the thought of this naturally inspires. His air, his manner, his deportment, all mark that elegant, and graceful sen se of his own superiority, which those who are bom to inferior sta tio n s can hardly ever arrive at. T h ese are the arts by which he proposes to make mankind more easily submit to h is authority, and to govern their inclinations according to h is own pleasure; and in th is he is seldom disappointed. T hese arts, supported by rank and preeminence, are, upon ordinary o ccasion s, sufficient to govern the world. 1 If such virtuosi actually exist, they would provide a suitable group in which to study the techniques by which activity is transformed into a show. Idealization It w as suggested earlier that a performance of a routine presents through its front some rather abstract claim s upon the audience, claim s that are likely to be presented to them during the performance of other routines. T his co n stitu tes one way in which a performance is , in a sen se, 's o c ia lis e d ,’ moulded and modified to fic into the understanding and expectations l Adam Smith, The Theory o f Moral Sentim ents (L o n do n: Henry Bohn, 1853), p . 75. 22 of the society in which it is prt-senred. V!'e consider here another important aspect of this socialization p ro cess—the tendency for performers to offer their observers an impression that is idealized in several different ways. T he notion that a performance presents an idealized view of the situation is, of course, quite common. C ooley's view may be taken a s an illustration: U we never cried to seem a little better than we are, how could we improve or 'train ourselves from the outside inward?' And the siituc impulse to show the world a better or idealized a sp ect of ourselves finds an organized expression in the various professions and cla s se s , cacli of which has to some exten t a cant or pose» which its members lissome unconsciously, for the most part, but which has the effect of ,t conspiracy to work upon the credulity of (he rest of the world. There is a cant not only of theology and of philanthropy, but also of law, medicine, teaching, even of scien cc—perhaps especially of science, just now since the more u particular kind of merit is recognizcd and admired, the more it Is likely to be assum ed by the unworthy.1 Thus, when the individual presents himself before others, liis performance will tend to incorporate and exemplify the officially accredited values ot the society, more so, in fact, limn does his behaviour as a whole. To the degree that a performance highlights the common official values of the society in which it occurs, >we may look upon it, in the manner of Durkheim and 1' the Kasic unit. It has been suggested that the object of a perlormer is to sustain a particular definition of the situation, this representing, as it were, his claim as to what reality is. As a one-man team, with no team-mates to inform of his decision, he can quickly decide which-of the available stands on a matter to take and then wholeheartedly act as if his choice were the only one he could possibly have taken. And his choice of position may be nicely adjusted to his own particular situation. When we turn front a one-man team to a larger one, the character of the reality that is espoused by the team changes. Instead of a rich definition of the situation, reality may become reduced to a thin party line, for we may expect the line to be unequally congenial to the members of the team. We may expect ironic remarks by which a team-mate jokingly rejects the line while seriously accepting it. On th e . other hand, there will be the new factor of loyalty to one’s team and one’s team-mates to provide support for the team’s line. It seems to be generally felt that public disagreement among the members of the team not only incapacitates them lor united action but also em barrasses the reality sponsored by the team. To protect this impression of reality, members of the team may be required to postpone taking public stands until the position of the team has been settled; and once the team’s stand has been taken, all members may be obliged to follow it. 1 An illustration may be taken from the civil se rv ic e : Ac such committees (Cabinet Committee meetings) civil servants share iu the discussions and evprcss their views freely, subject to one qualification : they will not directly oppose theit own Minister. The possibility of such open disagreement very rarely arises, and ou^ht never co 3 r i s e : in nine c a s e s out of Len, the Minister and the c ivil servant who attends the cjnm itcce with him have agreed beforehand what line is ro be taken, and in che tenth the civil servant who disagrees with his M in isters view on a particular point will stay 3wav from the meeting where it is co be discussed. 2 Another illustration may he cited from a recent study power structure of a smnlL city: If one has been en^a^cd in community work on any scale at all, Si: is impressed over and over with whac might be termed the ’ principle ol unanimity.’ \Mien policy is finally formulated by the leaders in the community, chere is an immediace demand on their part for strict conformity of opinion. Oecisions are not usually arrived at hutriedly. There is ample time, parcicularly among (he top leaders, lor discussion of most projects before a state of action is set. This is true for community projects. When the time for discussion is past and che line is set, rhen unanimity is called for. P ressures are put upon 1 I’he question of the amount of ’Soviet self-criticism* that is allowed, and from whom it is allowed, before che team 's position is announced is not here at issue. 2 Pale, op. cit , p. 141. 53 dissen ters, and the project is under way. 1 However, unanimity is often not the sole requirement of the team’s projection. There seems to be a general feeling that the most real and solid things in life are ones whose description individuals independently agree upon. We tend to feel that if two participants in an event lec'ide to be as honest a s they can in recounting it, then the stands they take will be acceptably similar even though they do not consult ode another prior to their presentation. Intention to tell the truth presumably makes such prior consultation unnecessary. And we also tend to feel that if the two individuals wish to tell a lie or to slant the version of the event which they offer, then not only will it be necessary for them to consult with one another in order, as we say, ' to get their story straight,’ but it will also be necessary to conceal the fact that an opportunity for such prior consultation was available to them. In other words, in staging a definition of the situation, it may be necessary for the several members of the team to be unanimous in the positions they take and secretive about the fact that these positions were noc independently arrived at. (Incidentally, if the members of the team are a ls o engaged in maintaining a show of self-respect before one another, it may be necessary for the members of the team to learn what the line is to be, and take it, without admitting to them selves and to one another the extent to which cheir position is not independently arrived at, but such problems carry us somewhat beyond the teamperformance a s the basic point of reference.) It should be noted that just as a team-mate ought to wait 1 Floyd iiunter, Community Power Structure (Chapel Hill: University of Notch Carolina P re ss, 1953)» p. 181. See also p. 118 and p. 212. Open disagreement in front of the audience creates, as we say, a false note. It may be suggested that literal false notes arc avoided tor quite the sam e reasons that figurative false notes are avoided; in both c a s e s it i s a matter of su staining a definition of the situation. This may be illustrated from a brief book on the work problems of the professional concert-nrtist accompanist, Gerald Moore, fh e Unashamed Accompanist (New York: Macmillan, 1944), p* 60: * The nearest that she singer and pianist can get to an ideal performance is to do cxactly what the composer wants, yet sometimes rhe singer will require his partner to do something which is in flat contradiction to the composer's markings, lie will want an accent where there should be none, he will make a firmala where it is not needed, he w»ll make a rallentando when it should be a tempo', he will be forte when,he should be p ia n o: he may sentim entalize when the mood should be nobilmente. ‘ The list is by no means exhausted. The singer will swear with h is hand on his heart and tears in his eyes that he does and always aim s to do exactly what the composer has written. It is very awkward. If he sings it one way and the pianist plays it another way the result is chaotic. Discussion may be of no avail. Dut what is an accompanist to do ? ' At the performance he must be with the singer, but afterwards let him erase the memory of it from his mind . . .' 54 for the official woH before taking his stan.l, so the official word ought to be made available to him so that he can play his part on the team and feel a part of it. To withhold from a teaii-mate information about the stand his team is taking i s to withhold his character from him, for without knowing what stand he will be taking he may not be able to assert a self to the audience. Thus, if a surgeon is to operate on a patient referred to him by another doctor, common courtesy may oblige the surgeon to tell the referring doctor when the operation will be and, if the referring doctor does not appear at the operation, to telephone him the result of the operation. By thus being 'filled in,’ the referring doctor can, more effectively than otherwise, present himself to the patient’s kinsfolk as someone who is participating in the medical action. 1 I would like to add a further general fact about maintaining the line during a performance. When a member of the team makes a mistake in the presence of the audience, we often find that the other team members must suppress their immediate desire to punish and instruct the offender until, that is, the audience is no longer present. After all, immediate corrective sanctioning would often only disturb the interaction further and, a s previously suggested, make the audience privy to n view that ought to be reserved for team-mates. Thus, tn authoritarian organizations, where a team of superordinates maintains a show of being right every time and of possessing a united front, there is often a strict rule that one superordinate must not show hostility or disrespect toward any other superordinate while in the presence of a member of the subordinate team. Army officers show consensus when before enlisted men, parents when before children, 2 managers when before workers, nurses when before patients, 3 and the like. Of course, when the subordinates are absent, open, violent 1 fn commenting on how some Chinese merchants set the price of iheit goods according to the appearance of the customer, Chester Holcombe, The R eal Chinaman (New York: Dodd, Mead, 1895), p. 293, goes on to say : 'O n e peculiar result of this study of a customer is seen in the fact that if a person enters a store in China, and, after examining several articles, asks the price of any one of them, unless it is positively known that he has spoken to but one clerk, no answer will be made by him to whom the question is put until every other clerk has been asked if he lias named a price for the article in question io the gentleman. If, a s very rarely happens, this important precaution Is neglected, the sum named by different clerks will almost invariably be unlike, thus showing that they fail to agree in their estim ates of the customer.* 2 An interesting dramaturgical difficulty in the family is that sex and lineal solidarity, 'which cross-cut conjugal solidarity, make it difficult for husband and wife to 'b a c k each other up ’ in a show of authority before children or a show of either distance or familiarity with extended kin. As previously suggested, such cross-cutting lines of affiliation prevent the widening of structural cleavages. 3 Taxel, op. eil., pp. 53-54. 55 criticism may and does occur. For example, in a recent study of the teaching profession, it was found that teachers felt that if they are to sustain an impression of professional competence and institutional authority, they must make sure that when angry parents come to the school with complaints, the principal will support the position of his staff, at least until the parents h a /e left. 1 Similarly, teachers feel strongly chat their fellowteachers ought not to disagree with or contradict them in front of students. 'J u s t let another teacher raise her eyebrow funny, just so they (the children) know, and they don’t miss a tiling, and their respect for you goes right away.’ 2 Similarly, we learn that the medical profession has a strict code of etiquette whereby a consultant in the presence of the patient and his doctor is careful never to say anything which would embarrass the impression of competence that the patient’s doctor is attempting to maintain. As Hughes suggests, 'T h e (professional) etiquette is a body of ritual which grows up informally to preserve, before the clients, the common front of the profession.’ 3 And, of course, this kind of solidarity in the presence of subordinates also occurs when performers are in the presence of superordinates. For example, in a recent study of the police we leam that a patrolling team of two policemen, who witness each other’s illegal and semiillegal ac ts and who are in an excellent position to discredit each other’s show of legality before the judge, po ssess heroic solidarity and will stick by each other’s story no matter what atrocity it covers up or how little chance there is of anyone believing it. 4 It is apparent that if performers are concerned with maintaining a line they will select as team-mates those who can be trusted to perform properly. Thus children of the house are often excluded from performances given for guests of a domestic establishm ent because often cnil iren cannot be trusted to 'behave* them selves, i.e., to refrain from acting in a way inconsistent with the impr'ession that is being fostered. 6 In fact, children must often be excluded from 1Howard S. Becker, *The T eacher in the Authority System of the Public School,' Journal o f Educational Sociology, XXVII, 134. *!bi<(., from an interview, p. 139. 3 E. C .Hughes, 'Institutions,* New Outline o f the Principles o f Sociology, ed. Alfred M .L ee (New York : Barnes and Noble 1946), p. 273**William Westley, 'T h e P o lic e ' (Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1952), pp. 187-196. 5 In so far a s children are defined a s 'non-persons* they have some liccnce to commit gauche acts without requiring the audience to take the expressive im plications of these acts too seriously. However, 56 gossip and from adm issions on the par: of older members of the family, since one can never he sure to whom one’s children will convey one’s secrets, so that it will be only when the child arrives at the age of discretion that the voices of his parents will cease to drop as he enters the room. Similarly, those who are known to become intoxicated when drink is available and who become verbose or ' difficult ’ when chis occurs constitute a performance risk, as do those who are sober but foolishly indiscreet, and those who refuse to 'en te r into the sp irit’ of the occasion and help sustain the impression that the guests tacitly unite in maintaining to the host. 1 have suggested that in many interaction settings some of the participants co-operate together as a team or are in a position where they are dependent upon this co-operation in order to maintain a particular definition of the situation. Now when we study concrete social establishm ents we often find that there will be a significant sense in which all the remaining participants, in their several performances of response to the team-show put on before them, will themselves constitute a team. Since each team will be playing through its routine for the other, we may speak of dramatic interaction, not dramatic action, and we can see this interaction not as a medley of as many voices as there are participants but rather as a kind of dialogue and interplay between two teams. I do not know of any general reason why interaction in natural settings usually takes the form of two-team interplay, or is resolvable into this form, instead of involving a larger number, but empirically this seems to be the case. Thus, in large social establishm ents, where several different status grades prevail, we find that for the duration of any particular interaction, participants of many different statu ses are typically expected to align themselves temporarily into two team groupings. For example, a lieutenant on an Army post will in on1, situation find himself aligned with all the officers and opposed to all enlisted men; at other times he will find himself aligned with junior officers, presenting with them a show for the benefit of senior officers present. There are, of course, asp ects of certain interactions for which a two-team model is apparently not suitable. Important elements, for example, of arbitration hearings seem to fit a three-team model, and asp ects of some competitive and 's o c i a l ’ situations suggest a multi-team model. It should also be made clear that whatever the number of teams, there will be a se n se in which the whether treated as non-persons or not, children are in a position to disclose crucial secrets. 57 interaction can be analysed in terms of the co-operative effort of all participants to maintain a working consensus. If we treat an interaction as a dialogue between two teams, ic will sometimes be convenient to call one team the performers and to call the other team the audience or the observers, neglecting momentarily that the audience, too, will be presenting a team-performance. In some ca se s, as when two one-person teams interact in a public institution or in the home of a mutual friend, it may be an arbitrary choice a s to which team to call the performer and which to call the audience. In many important social situations, however, the social setting in which the interaction occurs is assem bled and managed by one of the team s only, and contributes in a more intimate way to the show this team puts on than to thfe show put on in response by the other team. A customer in a shop, a clienc in an office, a group of guests in the home of their h o sts—these persons put on a performance and maintain a front, but the setting in which they do this is outside of cheir immediate control, being an integral part of the presentation made by those into whose presence they have come. In such ca se s, it will often be convenient co call the team which controls the setting the performing team, and to call the other team the audience. So, too, it will sometimes be.convenient to label as performer the team which contributes the most activity co the interaction, or plays the more dramatically prominent part in it, or sets che pace and direction which both teams will follow in their interactive dialogue. The obvious point muse be stated chat if the team is co sustain che impression chat ic is fostering, then chere must be some assurance that no individual will be allowed to join boch team and audience. Thus, for example, if the proprietor of a small American la d ies’-ready-to-wear is to put a dress on sa le and tell his customers that it is marked down because of soilage, or end of the season, or last of a line, etc., and conceal from her that ic is really marked down because it won’t sell, or is a bad colour,- or style, and if he is ro impress her by calking about a buying office in New York which he does not have or an adjustment manager who is really a salesgirl, then he must make sure chac if he finds it necessary co hire an extra girl for part-time work on Saturday he does not hire one trom che neighbourhood who has been a customer and who will soon be one again. 1 It is often felt that control of the setting is an advantage 1 T hese Illustrations are taken from George Rosenbaum, 'An Analysis of Personalization in Neighbourhood Apparel R etailing’ (Unpublished M. A. th esis, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1953), pp. 86-87- 58 during interaction. la a narrow sense, this control allows a team to introduce strategic devices for determining the information the audience is able to acquire. Thus, if Joctors are to prevenc cancer patients from learning the identity of their disease, it will be useful to scatter the cancer patients throughout the hospital so that they will not be able to learn from the identity of their ward the identity of their disorder. (The hospital staff, incidentally, may be forced to spend more time walking corridors and moving equipment because of this staging strategy than would otherwise be necessary.) Similarly, the master barber who regulates the flow of appointments by means of a scheduling took open to his public is in a position to protect his coffee-break by filling a properlytimed appointment with a dummy code name. A prospective customer can then see for himself that it will not be possible for him to have an appointment at that time. 1 So, too, if any member of a hotel staff is suspicious of the intentions or character of a guest couple, a secret signal can be given to the bellboy to 'throw the latch .1 T h is is simply a device which makes it easier for employees to keep an eye on suspected parties. After rooming the couple, the bellman, in closing the door behind him, pushes a tiny button on the insiJc of the knob handle. This turns a little tumbler inside the lock and makes a black stripe show against the circular center of the latch on the outside. U*s inconspicuous enough so as not to be noticed by the guest, but maids, patrols, w aiters and bellmen are all trained to watch for them . . . and to report any loud conversations or unusual occurrences vyhich take place behind them. 2 More broadly, control of the setting may give the controlling team a sense of security. As one studenc suggestsconcerning the pharmacist-doctor relation: T he score is another factot. The doctor often comcs to the pharm acist's store for medicine, for b its of information, fot conversation. In these conversations the man behind the counter has approximately the same advantage that a standing speaker has ovet a sitting audience ^ i An interesting use of setting and props is reported in a newspaper article on sorotities, Joan Beck, *t h a t ’s \fcrong with Sorority Rushing?’ Chicago Tribune Magazine, January 10, 1954, pp. 20-21, where a description is given of how the sorority sisters, who give a tea for prospective members, are able to sort out good prospects ftom bad without giving the impression that guests of the house are being treated differentially: *u Even with recommends, it's hard to remember 967 girls by just meeting them for a few minutes in a receiving line,” admitted Carol. MSo we've worked out this gimmick to separate the good ones from the dull characters. Ae have three trays for the rushees' calling ca rd sone fot golden girls, one for look~agains, one for pots. '" T h e active who is talking with the rushee at the party is supposed to escort her subtly to the appropriate tray when s h e 's ready to lrav^ her card,” Carol continued. "T h e rushees never figure out what w e’re doing !-1 2 Dev Collans, with Stewart Sterling, / A House D etective (New York: Dutton, 1954), p. 56. E llipsis dots the author's. ^ e i n l e i n , op. cit., p. 105. 59 One thinj; chat contributes co this feeling of che independence of the pharm acist's medical practice is his store. The store is, in a sen se, a part of the pharmacist- Ju st as Neptune is pictured as rising from the sea, while a t the same time being che s e a ; so in the pharmaceutical ethos there is a vision of a dignified pharmacist tirwering above shelves and counters of bottles auu equipment, while at the same time being part of thoir essen ce . 1 A price must, of course, be paid for che privilege of giving a performance on one’s home ground; one has che opportunicy of conveying information abouc oneself through sc en ic means but no opportunity of concealing the kinds of faccs chac are conveyed by scenery. When we examine a ceam-performance, we often find that som eone is given che right to direct and control the progress of the dramatic action. The equerry in court establishm ents is an example. Sometimes the individual who dominates the show in this way and is, in a sense, the director of it, plays an actual part in the performance he directs. In general, the members of the team will differ in the ways and the degree to which they are allowed co direcc che performance. Ic may be noted, incidentally, that, dramaturgically sp e a k in g ,. the structural sim ilarities of apparently diverse routines are nicely reflected in the like-mindedness that arises in directors everywhere. Whether ic is a funeral, a wedding, a bridge party, a one-day sale, a hanging, or a picnic, •che director may tend to see the performance in terms of whether or not it went 'sm oothly,’ 'effe ctiv ely ,1 and 'w ithout a hitch,’ and 1Weinlein, op. cit., pp. 105-106. A nice literary illustration of the eifccts of being robbed of control over o n e s own setring i« given in l*ranz Kafka, The Trial (Ne-* York: KjTopf, 1948), pp. 14-15, where K .s meeting with the authorities' in his own boarding house is described: ’ VShen he was fully dressed he had to walk, with Uillem trending nn his heels, through the next room, which was now empty, into the adjoining one, whose double doors were flung open. This room, ns K. knew quite well, had tecencly l>ecn taken by a Fraulcin Uursrner, a typist, who went very early to work, came home lace, and with whom he hud exchanged little more dian few words in passing. Mow the nighttable beside her bed had been pushed into die middle of the floor to serve a« desk, and the Inspector was sitting behind it. lie had crosscd h is legs, and one arm was resting on the back of the chair. . . . . ’ "J o se p h K. ? ” asked the inspector, perhaps merely to draw K-’s distracted glance upon himself. K. nodded. "You are presumably very surprised a t the events of this m orning?" asked che inspector, with both hands rearranging the few things that lay on the oigltt-table, a candle and a matchbox, a book and a pincushion, as if they were objects which he required for h is interrogation. “ Certainly,n said K., and he was filled with pleasure at having encountered a sensible man at lasr, with whom he could d iscuss the matter. * Certainly, I am surprised, but I am by rro means very surprised.” "Not very s u r p r is e d ? ” asked the inspector, setting the candle in the middle of the table and dicn grouping the other things around it. “ Perhaps you misunderstand me,* K. hastened ro add. *[ m e a n " —here K. stopped and looked round him for a c h a ir—" I suppose 1 may sit down ? " he ask"d. " lt‘s not usual,” answered the Inspector.' 60 whether or not all possible disruptive contingencies were prepared for in advance. tn many performances two important functions must be fulfilled, and if the team has a director he will often be given the special duty of fulfilling these functions. First, the director may be given the special duty of bringing baclc into line any member of the team whose performance becomes unsuitable. Soothing and sanctioning are the corrective processes ordinarily involved. The role of the baseball umpire in sustaining a particular kind of reality for the fans may be taken as an illustration. All umpires in sist that players keep themselves under control, and refrain from gescures that reflect concempt for their decisions. 1 I certainly had blown off my share of steam as a player, and I knew there had to be a safety valve for release of the terrific tension. As nn umpire 1 could sympathize with the players. Gut as an umpire I had to decide how for I could let a player go without delaying the r.Amc and without permitting him to insult, assault, or ridicule me and belittle the game. Handling trouble and mea on the field was j s important as calling them tight—and more difficult. It is easy for any umpire to thumb a man out of the game. It is often a much more difficult job to keep him in the game—to understand and anticipate his complaint so that a nasty rhubarb cannot develop. 2 I do not tolerate clowning on the field, and neither will any other umpire. Comedians belong on the stage, or on television not in baseball. A travesty or burlesque of the game can only cheapen it, and also hold the umpire up to scorn- for allowing such a sketch to tak e place. T h at’s why you will see the funnymen and wise guys chased a s soon a s they begin their routine. 3 Often, of course, the director will not so much have to smother improper affect as he will have to stimulate a show of proper affective involvement; 'sparking the show ’ is the phrase sometimes employed for this cask in Rotarian circles. Secondly, the director may be given the special duty of allocating the parts in the performance and the personal front that is employed in each part, for each establishment may be seen as a place with a number of characters to dispose of to prospective performers and as an assem blage of sign equipment or ceremonial paraphernalia to be allocated. It is apparent that if the director corrects for improper appearances and allocates major and minor prerogatives, chen other members of the ream (who are likely to be concerned with the show they can put on for one another as well as with the show they can collectively stage for the audience) will have an attitude toward the director that they do not have toward their other team-mates. Further, if the audience 1Pinelli, op. cit., p. H I . 2 Ibid., p. 131. 3 I b id , p. 139. 61 appreciates that the performance has a Jirector, they are likely to hold him more responsible chan other performers for the s u c c e ss of the performance. The director is likely to respond to this responsibility by making dramaturgical demands on the performance that chey might not make upon them selves. This may add to the estrangement they may already feel from him. A director, hence, starting as a member of the team, may find himself slowly edged into a marginal role between audience and performers, half in and half out of both camps, a kind of go-between without the protection that go-betweens usually have. The factory foreman has been a recently discussed example. 1 When we study a routine which requites a team of several performers for its presentation, we sometimes find that one member of the team is made the star, lead, or centre of attention. We may see an extreme example of this in traditional court life, where a room full of court attendants will be arranged in the manner of a living tableau, s d that the eye, starting from any point in the room will be led to the royal centre of attention. The royal star of the performance may also be dressed more spectacularly and seated higher than anyone e lse present. An even more spectacular centring of attention may be found in the dance arrangements of large musical comedies, in which forty or fifty dancers are made to prostrate them selves around the herpine. In general, we find that those who help present a team-performance differ in the degree of dramatic dominance given each of them and that one team-routine differs from another in the extent to which differentials in dominance are given its members. T he conception of dramatic and directive dominance, as contrasting types of power in a performance, can be applied, mutatis mutandis, to an interaction a s a whole, where it will be possible to point out which of the two teams lias more of which of the two types of power anJ which performers, taking the participants of both teams all together, lead in these two regards. Frequently, of course, we may expect that the performer or team which has one kind of dominance is likely also to to have the other, but this is by no means always the case. F or example, during the showing oi the body at a funeral home, usually the social setting and all participants, in­ 1 See, for example, Donald E. Wray, 'Marginal Men of Industry: The Foreman,' American Journal of Sociology, LIV, pp. 298-301, and Fritz Koethlisberger, 'T h e torem an : Master and Victim of Double Talk,' Harvard B u siness Review, XXIII, pp. 285-294. The role of go-between i s considered later. 62 eluding both the bereaved team and the establishment’s team, will be arranged so as to express their feelings for and ties with the deceased; he will be the centre of the show and the dramatically dominant participant in it. However, since the bereaved are inexperienced and grief-laden, and since the star of the show must stay in character a s someone who is in a deep sleep, the undertaker himself will direct the show, although he may all the while be self-effacing in th e presence of the corpse or be in another room of the establishment getting ready for another showing. It should be made clear that dramatic and directive dominance are dramaturgical terms and that performers who enjoy such dominance may not have other types of power and authority. It is common knowledge that performers who have positions of visible leadership are often merely figureheads, selected as a compromise, or as a way of neutralizing a potentially threatening position, or as a way of strategically concealing the power behind the front and hence the power behind the power behind the front. So also, whenever inexperienced or temporary incumbents are given formal authority over experienced subordinates, we often find that the formally empowered person is bribed with a part that has dramatic dominance while the subordinates tend to direct the show. 1 T hus it has often been said about the British Infantry in World War I that experienced working-class sergeants managed the delicate task of covertly teaching their new lieutenants to take a dramatically expressive role at the head of the platoon and to die quickly in a prominent dramatic position, as befits Public School men. The sergeants themselves took their modest place at the rear of the platoon and tended to live to train still other lieutenants. Dramatic and directive dominance have been mentioned as two dimensions along which each place on a team can vary. By changing the point of reference a little, we can discern a third mode of variation. In general, those who participate in the activity that occurs in a social establishm ent become members of a team when they co-operate together to present their activity in a particular light. However, in taking on the role of a performer, the individual need not cease to devote some of his effort to non-dramaturgical concerns, that is, to the activity itself of which the performance offers an acceptable dramatization. ' See David Riesman, in collaboration with Reuel Denny and Nathan Glaser, The Lonely Crowd (New Haven: Ynle University P ress, 1950), *The Avocacional Counselors,' pp. 363-367- 63 We may expect, then, that the individuals who perform on a particular team will differ among them selves in the way they apportion their time between mere activity an I mere performance. At one extreme we find individuals who rarely appear before the audience and are little concerned with appearances. At the other extreme we find what are sometimes ca 'le d 'purely ceremonial ro les,’• whose performers will be concerned with the appearance that they make, and concerned with little else. For example, the president and the research director of a national union may both spend time in the main office of the union headquarters, •appearing suitably dressed and suitably spoken in order to give the union a front of respectability. However we may find that the president also engages in making many important decisions whereas the research director may have little to do except be present in body as part of the president’s retinue. Union officials conceive of such purely ceremonial roles as part of 'windowdressing.' 1 It may be remarked that an individual with a purely ceremonial role need not have a dramatically dominant one. ammaro A team, then, may be defined as a set of individuals whose intimate co-operation is required if n given projected definition of the situation is to be maintained. A team is a grouping, but it is a grouping not in relation to a social structure or social organization but rather in relation to an interaction or series of interactions in which the relevant definition of the situation is maintained. We have seen, and will see further, that if n performance is to be effective it will be likely that the extent and character of the co-operation that makes this possible will be concealed and kept secret. A team, then, has something ol the character of a secret society. • Thfe audience may appreciate, of course, that all the members of the team are held together by a bond no member of the audience shares. Thus, for example, when customers enter a service establishment, they clearly appreciate that all employees are different from customers by virtue of this official role. However, the individuals who 'S e e Harold L. Wilensky, 'T h e Staff "E x p e rt:" A Study of the Intelligence Function in American Trade U nions' (Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1953), chap. iv. in addition to his th esis material, 1 am indebted Mr Wilensky for many suggestions. 64 are on the staff of an establishment are not members of a team by virtue of staff status, but only by virtue of the cooperation which they maintain in order to sustain a given definition of the situation. No effort may be made in many c a se s to conceal who is on the staff; but they form a secret socicty, a team, in so far as a secret is kept as to how they are co-operating together to maintain a particular definition of the situation. Teams may be created by individuals to aid the group they are members of, but in aiding themselves and their group in this dramaturgical way, they are acting as a team, not a group. T hus a team, as used herein, is the kind of secret society whose members may be known by noninembers to constitute a society, even an exclusive one, but the society these individuals are known to constitute is not the one they constitute by virtue of acting as a team. 65 C H A P T E R HI REGIONS AND REGION BEHAVIOUR A region may be defined as any place that is bounded to some degree by barriers to perception. Regions vary, of course, in the degree to which they are bounded and according to the media of communication in which the barriers to perception occur. Thus thick glass panels, such as are found in broadcasting control rooms, can isolate a region aurally but not visually, while an office bounded by beaverboard partitions is closed off in the opposite way. In our Anglo-American society—a relatively indoor one— when a performance is given it is usually. given in a highly bounded region, to which boundaries with respect to time are often added. The impression and understanding fostered by the performance will then tend, as it were, to saturate the region and time span, so that any individual located in this space-tim e manifold will be in a position to observe the performance and be guided by the definition of the situation which the performance fosters. 1 Often a performance will involve only one focus of visual attention on the part of performer and audience, a s , for example, when a political speech is presented in a hall or when a patient is talking to a doctor in the latter’s consultingroom. However many performances involve, as constituent parts, seperate knots or clusters of verbal interaction. T hus a cocktail party typically involves several conversational sub-groups which constantly shift in size and membership. So, too, the show maintained on the floor of a shop typically involves several foci of verbal interaction, each composed of attendant-customer pairs. Given a particular performance as a point of reference, it will sometimes be convenient to use the term 'front region’ to refer to the pl?ce where the performance is given. The fixed sign-equipment in such a place has already been referred to as that part of front called 's e ttin g .' We will have to see that some asp ects of a performance seem to be played not to the audience but to the front region. 1Under the term 'behavioural se tting,’ Wright and Barker, in a research methodology report, give a very clear statement of the se n se s in which expectations regarding conduct come to be associated with particular p laces. See Herbett F . Wright and Roger G. Barker, Methods in Psychological Ecology (Topeka: K an sas: Ray’s Printing Service, 1950). 66 The performance of an individual in a front region may be seen as an effort to give the appearance that his activity in the region maintains and embodies certain standards. These standards seem to fall into two broad groupings. One grouping has to do with the way in which the performer treats the audience while engaged in tall: with them or in gestural interchanges that are a substitute for talk. These standards are sometimes referred to as matters of politeness. The other group of standards has to do with the way in which the performer comports himself while in visual or aural range of the audience but not necessarily engaged in talk with them. I shall use the term decorum to refer to this second group of standards, although some excuses and some qualifications will have to be added to justify the usage. Uhen we look at the requirements of decorum in a region, requirements of the kind not related to the handling of others in conversation, we tend to divide these again into two subgroupings, moral and instrumental. Moral requirements are ends in them selves and presumably refer to rules regarding non-interference and non-molestation of others, rules regarding sexual propriety, rules regarding respect for sacred places, etc. Instrumental requirements are not ends in themselv es and presumably refer to duties such as an employer might demand of his employe.es—care of property, maintenance of work levels, etc. It may be felt chat the term decorum ought to cover only the moral standards and that another term should be used to cover the instrumental ones. When we examine the order that is maintained in a given region, however, we find that these two kinds of demands, moral and instrumental, seem to affect in much the same way the individual who must answer to them, and that both moral and instrumental grounds or rationalization are put forth as justifications for most standards that must be maintained. Providing the standard is maintained by sanctions and by a sanctioner of some kind, it will often be of small moment to the performer whether the standard is justified chiefly on instrumental grounds or moral ones, and whether he is asked to incorporate the standard. It may be noted that the part of personal front we have called 'm an n e r’ will be important in regard to politeness and that the part we have called ’ appearance’ will be important in regard to decorum. It may also be noted that while decorous behaviour may take the form of showing respect for the region and setting one finds oneself in, this show of respect may, of course, be motivated by a desire to impress 67 the audience favourably, or avoid sanctions, etc. Finally, it should be noted that the requirements of decorum are more pervasive ecologically than are the requirements of politeness. An audience can subject an entire front region to a continuous inspection as regards decorum, but while the audience is so engaged, none or only a few of the performers may be obliged to talk to the audience and hence to demonstrate politeness. In the study of social establishm ents it is important to describe the standards of decorum; it is difficult to do so because informants and students tend to take many of these standards for granted not realizing they have done so until an accident, or crisis, or peculiar circumstance occurs. VC’e know, for example, that different business offices have different standards as regards informal chatter among clerks, f'Ut it is only when we happen to study an office that has a sizeable number of foreign refugee employees that we suddenly appreciate that permission to engage in informal talk may not constitute permission to engage in informal talk in a foreign language. 1 We are accustomed to assuming that the rules of decorum that prevail in sacred establishm ents, such as churches, will be much different from the ones that prevail in everyday places of work. We ought not to assume from this that die standards in sacred places are more numerous and more s m c t than those we find in work establishm ents. While in church, a woman may be permitted to sit, daydream, and even doze; a s a saleswoman on the floor of a dress shop, she may be required to stand, keep alert, refrain from chewing gum, keep a fixed smile on her face even when not talking to anybody and wear clothes she can ill afford. One form of decorum that has been studied in social establishm ents is what is called ’ make-work.’ It is understood in many establishm ents that not only will workers be required to produce a certain amount after a certain length of time but also that they will be ready, when called upon, to give the impression that they are working hard at the moment. Of a shipyard we learn the following: It was amusing to watch the sudden transformation whenever word KOt round that the foreman was on the hull or in the shop or that a ftont-office superintendent was coming by. ‘Juarcermen and lcadermen would rush to their groups of workers and stir them to obviuus activity. ’ l)on\ let him catch you sitting down,’ was the universal admonition, and where no work existed a pipe was busily bent and threaded, or a bolt which was already firmly in place was sub- 1See Gross, op. cit., p. 186. 68 j e n e J to further and unnecessary tightening. T his * a s the formal tribute invariably attending a visitation by rhe boss, und its conventions were as familiar to both sid es as ihose surrounding a five-star general's inspection. To have neglected any detail of rhe false and empty sh'*>*would have been interpreted a s a mark- of singular disrespect. 1 Similarly, of o hospital ward we learn: The observer was told very explicitly by other attendants on his first day of work on the wards not to ‘ get caug ht’ striking a patient i to appear busy when tlie supervisor makes her rounds, and not to speak to her unless first spoken to. (c was noted that some attendants watch for her approach and warn the other attendants so that no one will gct c:iu£hr doing undesirable arts. Some attendants will save work for when the supervisor is present so they will be busy and will not be #ivcn additional tasks. In most attendants the change is not so obvious, depending largely on the individual attetidant, rhe supervisor, and the ward situation. Ifowevet, with nearly alt attendants there is some change in behaviour when an official, such a s a supervisor, is presenr. There is no open flouting of the rules and regulations................... ^ From a consideration of make-work it is only a step to consideration of other standards of work activity for which appearances must be maintained, such as pace, personal interest, economy, accuracy, etc. 3 And from a consideration of work standards in general it is only a step to consideration of other major asp ects of decorum, instrumental and moral, in work places, such a s : mode of d ress; permissible sound le v e ls ; proscribed diversions, indulgences, and affective expressions; etc. It was .suggested earlier that when one’s activity occurs in the presence of other persons, some asp ects of the activity are expressively accentuated and other aspects, which mi.tjht discredit the fostered impression, are suppressed. It is d e a r that accentuated facts make their appearance in what we have called a front region; it should be just as clear that there may be another region—a back region or backstage— where the suppressed facts make an appearance. A back region or backstage may be defined as a place, relative to a given performance, where the impression fostered by the performance is knowingly contradicted as a matter of course. There are, of course, many characteristic functions of such places. It is here that the capacity of a performance to express something beyond itself may be painstakingly fabricated; it is here that illusions and im pressions are openly constructed. Here grades of ceremonial equipment, such as different types of liquor or clothes, can be hidden 1Katherine Archibald, Wartime Shipyard (Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California P ress, 1947), p. 159- 2 Willoughby, op. cit., p. 4 3. ^An analysis of some major work standards may be found in Gross, op. c il,t from which the above examples of such standards are taken. 69 so that the audience will not be able to see the treatment accorded them in comparison with the treatment that could have been accorded them. Here devices such as the telephone are sequestered so that users will be able to use them 'p riv a te ly .’ Here costumes and other parts of personal front may be adjusted and scrutinized for flaws. Here the team can run through its performance, checking for offending expressions when no one is present to be affronted by them; here poor members of the team, who are expressively inept, can be schooled or dropped from the performance. Here the performer can relax; he can drop his front, forgo speaking his lines, and step out of character. Simone de Beauvoir provides a rather vivid picture of this backstage activity in describing situations from which the male audience is absent. What gives value to such relations among women is rhe truthfulness they imply. Confronting man woman is always play-acting; she lies when she makes believe that she accepts her status as the inessential other, she lies when she presents to him an imaginary personage through mimicry, costumery, studied phrases. T hese histrionics require a constant ten sio n : when with her husband, or with her lover, every woman is more or le s s conscious of the thought: 'I am not being myself^* the male world is harsh, sharp edged, its voices arc too resounatng, the lights ate too crude, the contacrs rough. With other women, a woman i s behind the s c e n e s ; she is polishing her equipment, but not in b attle; she is getting her costume together, preparing her make-up, laying out her ta c tic s ; she is lingering in dressing-gown and slippers in the wings before making her entrance on the stag e; she likes this warm, easy, relaxed atmosphere . . . . For some women this warm and frivolous intimacy is dearer than the serious pomp of relations with men. 1 Very commonly the back region of a performance is located at one end of the place where the performance is presented, being cut off from it by a partition and guarded passageway. By having the front and back regions adjacent in this way, a performer out in front can receive backstage assistan ce while the performance is in progress and can interrupt his performance momentarily for brief periods of relaxation. In general, of course, the back region will be the place where the performer can reliably expect that no member of the audience will intrude. Since the vital se crets of a show are visible backstage and since performers behave out of character while there, it is natural to expect that the passage from the front region to the back region will be kept closed to members of the audience or that the entire back region will be kept hidden from them. This is a widely practised technique of impression management, of which some illustrations and implications are given below. First, we often find that control of backstage plays a significant role in the process of 'work control’ whereby l de Beauvoir, op. ciL, p. 54 J. 70 individuals attempt to buffer them selves from the deterministic demands that surround them. If a factory worker is to succeed in giving the appearance of working hard all day, then he must have a safe place to hide the jig that enables him to turn out a day’s work with le ss than a full day’s effort. 1 If the bereaved are to be given the illusion that the dead one is really in a deep and tranquil sleep, then the undertaker must be able to keep the bereaved from the workroom where the corpses are drained, stuffed, and painted in preparation for their final performance.2 In many service trades, the customer is asked to leave the thing that needs service and to go away so that the tradesman can work in private. When the customer returns for his automobile—or watch, or trousers, or w ireless—it is presented to him in good working order, an order that incidentally conceals the amount and kind of work that had to be done, the number of m istakes that were first made before getting it fixed, and other details the client would have to know before being able to judge the reasonableness of the fee that is asked of him. Service personnel so commonly take for granted the right to keep the audience away from the back region that attention is drawn more to ca se s where this common strategy cannot be applied than to ca se s where it can. For example, the American filling station manager has numerous troubles in this regard. 3 If a repair is needed, customers often refuse to leave their automobile overnight or all day, in trust of the establishment, as they would do had they taken their automobile to a garage, Further, when the mechanic makes repairs and adjustments, customers often feel they have the right to watch him as he does his work. If an illusionary service is to be rendered and charged for, it must, therefore,- be rendered before the very person who is to be taken in by it. Customers, in fact, not only disregard the right of the station personnel to their own back region but often also define the whole station as a kind of open city for males, -a place where an individual runs the risk of getting his suit dirty and therefore 1See Orvis Collins, Melville Dalton, and Donald Koy, *Restriction of Output and Social Cleavage in Industry,' Applied Anthropology (now Human Organization), IV, pp. 1-1*1, esp. p. 9. 2Mr. liabcnstein has suggested tn seminar that in some sta te s the undertaker has a legal right to prevent relatives of the deceased from entering the workroom where the corpse is in preparation. Presumably the .sight of what has to be done to the dead to make them look attractive would be roo great a shock for non-professionals and especially for kinsfolk ol tin* deceased. Mr. Habenstein also suggests that kinsfork may want to be kept from the undertaker's workroom because of their own fear of iheir own morbid curiosity. ^ T h e statements which follow are taken from a study by Social Research Inc. of two hundred sm all-business managers. 71 has the right to demand full backstage privileges. Male m otorists will saunter in, tip back their hats, spit, swear, and ask for free service or free travel advice. They will barge in to make familiar use of the toilet, the station’s tools, or the office telephone; and in order to avoid traffic lights, motorists will cut right across the station driveway, oblivious to the manager’s proprietary rights. The study of the island hotel previously cited provides another example of the problems workers face when they have insufficient control of their backstage. Within the hotel kitchen, where the gu ests’ food was prepared and where the staff ate and spent their day, crofters’ culture tended to prevail, involving a characteristic pattern of clothing, food habits, table manners, language, employer-employee relations, cleanliness standards, etc. T his culture was felt to be different from, and lower in esteem than, British m iddle-class culture, which tended to prevail in the dining room and other places in the hotel. The. doors leading from the kitchen to the other parts of the hotel were a constant sore spot in the organization of work. The maids wanted to keep the doors open to make it easier to carry food trays back and forth, to gather information about whether guests were ready or not for the service which was to be performed for them, and to retain as much contact as possible with the persons they had come to work to learn about. Since the maids played a servant role before the guests, they, felt they did not have too much to lose by being observed in their own milieu by guests who glanced into the kitchen when passing the open doors. The managers, on the other hand, wanted to keep the door closed so that the m iddle-class role imputed to them by the guests would not be discredited by a disclosure of their crofter habits. Hardly a day passed when these doors were not angrily banged shut and angrily banged open. A kick-door of the kind modern restaurants use would have provided a partial solution for the staging problem. A small g la ss window in the doors that could act as a peephole—a stage device used by many small p laces of business—would also have been helpful. Another interesting example of backstage difficulties is found in radio and television broadcasting work. In these situations, back region tends to be defined' a s all places where the camera is not focussed at the moment or all places out of range of ' l i v e ’ microphones. Thus an announcer may hold the sponsor’s product up at arm’s length in front of the camera while he holds his nose with his other hand, his face being out of the picture, as a way of joking with his teammates. Professionals, of course, tell many exemplary tales 72 of how peisons who ihoughr they were backstage were in facc on the air and how this backstage conduct discredited the definition of the situation being maintained on the air. For technical reasons, then, the walls that broadcasters have to hide behind can be very treacherous, tending to fall at the flick of a switch or a turn ot the camera. Broadcasting artists must live with this staging contingency, A final example of backstage difficulties is found among exalted persons. P ersons may become so sacred that the only fitting appearance they can make is in the centre of a retinue and ceremony; it may be thought improper for them to appear before others in any other context, as such informal appearances may be thought to discredit the magical attributes imputed to them. Therefore members of the audicnce must be prohibited from all the places the exalted one is likely to relax in, and if the place for relaxation is large, as in the case of the Chinese Emperor in the nineteenth century, or it there is uncertainty about where the exalted one will be, problems of trespass become considerable. Thus Queen Victoria enforced the rule that anyone seeing her approach when driving in her pony-cart on the palace grounds should turn his head or walk in another direction, and sometimes great statesm en were required to sacrifice their own dignity and jump behind the shrubbery when the queen unexpectedly approached. 1 While these examples of back region difficulty are extreme, it would seem that no social establishment can be studied where some problems associated with backstage control do not occur. Work and recreation regions represent two areas for backstage control. Another area is suggested by the very widespread tendency in our society to give performers control over the place in which they attend to what are called biological needs. In our society, defecation involves an individual in activity which is defined as inconsistent with the cleanliness and purity standards expressed in many of our performances. Such activity also causes the individual to disarrange his clothing and to 'g o out of play,* that is, to drop from his face the expressive mask that he employs in face-to-face interaction. At the same time ic becomes difficult for him to reassem ble his personal front should the need to enter into interaction suddenly occur. Perhaps that is a reason why toilet doors in our society have locks on them. When asleep in bed the individual is also immobilized, * t'o n so n h y, of>. c i t . , p J2. 73 expressively speaking, and may not be able to bring himself into an appropriate position for interaction or bring a sociable expression to his face until some moments after being wakened, thus providing one explanation of the tendency to remove the bedroom from the active part of the house. The utility of such seclusion is reinforced by the facc that sexual activity is likely to occur in bedrooms, a form of interaction which also renders its performers incapable of immediately entering into an ocher interaction. One of the most interesting times to observe impression management is the moment when a performer leaves the back region and enters the place where the audience is to be found, or when he returns therefrom, for at these moments we can detect a wonderful putting on and taking off of character. Orwell, speaking of waiters, and speaking from the backstage point of view of dishwashers, provides us with an exam ple: 1c is an instructive sigh: co see a waiter going into a hotel dining-room. As he p a s s e s the door a sudden change comes over him. The set of h is shoulders a lte rs; all the dirt and hurry and irritation have dropped off in an instant. He glides over the carpec, with a solemn priest-like air. I remember our assista n t maitre d*hoielt a fiery Italian, pausing at the dining-room door to address his apprentice who had broken a bottle of wine. Shaking his fist above his head he yelled (luckily the door was more or le s s soundproof), ” Tu me fais — Do you call yourself a waiter, you young bastard? You a waiterl You re not fit to scrub floors in ihe brothel your mother came from. Mttffuereau! M Words failing him, he turned to the door; and a s he opened it lie delivered a final insult in the same manner a s Squire Uesrcrn in Tom Jones. Then he entered the dining-room and sailed across il dish in hand, graceful as a swan. Ten seconds loiter he was bowing reverently to a customer. And you could not help thinking, a s ^ou saw him bow and smile, with that benign smile of the trained waiter, that rho customer was put to shame by having such an aristocrat co serve him. 1 The decline of domestic service lias forced quick changes of the kind mentioned by Orwell upon the middle-class housewife. In serving a dinner for friends she must manage the kitchen dirty work in such a way as to enable her co switch back and forth between the roles of domestic and hostess, altering her activity, her manner, and her temper, as she 1George Orwell, Down and Out in Paris and London (London.* Seeker and Warburg, 1951), pp. 68-69* Another illustration is provided by Moqica Dickens, One Pair o f Hands (Mermaid Books; London: Michael Joseph, 1952), p. 13: ’ T h e said maid—her name was Addie, 1 discovered—and the two w aitresses were behaving like people acting in a play. They would sw eep into the kitchen a s if coming off stage into the wings, with trays held high and a tense expression of hauteur still on their faces; relax for a moment in the frenzy of getting the new dishes loaded, and glide off again with faces prepared to make their next entrance. The cook and 1 were left like stage hands among rhe debris, as if having.seen a glimpse of another world, we almost listened for the applause of the unseen audience.1 74 p a s s e s in and oiic of the dining room. Etiquette provide helpful directions for facilitating such changes. The line dividing front and back regions is illustrated everywhere in our society. As suggested, the bathroom and bedroom, in all but lower-class homes, are places from which the downstairs audience can be excluded. Bodies that are cleansed, clothed, and made up in these rooms can be presented to friends in others. In the kitchen, of course, there is done to food what in the bathroom and bedroom is done to the human body. It is, in fact, the presence of these staging devices that distinguishes middle-class living from lowercla ss living. But in all c la s s c s in our society there is a tendency to make a division between the front and back parts of residential exteriors. The front tends to be relatively well decorated, well repaired, and tidy; the rear tends to be relatively unprepossessing. Correspondingly, social adults enter through the front, and often the socially incomplete— domestics, delivery men, and children—enter through the rear. While we are familiar with the stage arrangements in and around a dwelling place, we tend to be le ss aware of other stage arrangements. In American residential neighbourhoods, boys of eight to fourteen and other profane persons appreciate that entrances to back lanes and alleys lead somewhere and are to be used; they see these openings in a vivid sense that will be lost to them when they become older. Similarly, janitors and scrubwomen have a clear perception of the small doors that lead to the back regions of business buildings and are intimately familiar wich the profane transportation system for secretly transporting dirty cleaning equipment, large stage props, and themselves. There is a similar arrangement in stores, where places 'behind the counter’ and the storeroom serve as back regions. Given the values of a particular society, it is apparent that the backstage character of certain places is built into them in a material way, and that relative to adjacent areas these places are inescapably back regions. In our society the decorator’s art often does this for us, apportioning dark colours and open brickwork to the service parts of buildings and white plaster to the front regions. P ieces of fixed equipment add permanency to this division. Employers complete the harmony by hiring persons with undesirable visual attributes for back region work,-1 placing persons who ‘niak? a good appearance’ in the front regions. (This involves a 1Reserves of unimpressive-looking labour can be used not only for activity that must be concealed from the audience but also for activity that need not but can be concealed. Mr Hughes has suggested in seminar 7S kind of ecological sorting that is well known but little studied.) And often it is expected that those who work backstage will achieve technical standards while those who work in the front region will achieve expressive ones. The decorations and permanent fixtures in a place where a particular performance is usually given, as well as the performers and performance usually found in it, tend to fix a kind of spell over it; even when the customary performance is not being given in it, the place tends to retain some of its front region character. Thus a cathedral and a schoolroom retain something of their tone even when only repairmen are present, and while these men may not behave reverently while doing their work, their irreverence tends to be of a structured kind, specifically oriented to what in some sense they ought to be feeling but are not. So, too, a given place may become so identified as a hide-out where certain standards need not be maintained that it becomes fixed with an identity as a back region. Hunting lodges and locker rooms in athletic social establishm ents may serve as illustrations. Summer resorts, too, seem to fix perm issiveness regarding front, allowing otherwise conventional people to appear in public stree ts in costumes they would not ordinarily wear in the presence of strangers. So, too, criminal hangouts and even criminal neighbourhoods are to be found, where the act of being ’ le g it’ need not be maintained. An interesting example of this is said to have existed in P a ris: ’ In the seventeenth century, therefore, in order to become a thorough Argotier, it was necessary not only to solicit alms like any mere heggar, but also to p o s s e s s the dexterity of the cut-purse and the thief. T hese arts were to be learned in the places which served a s the habitual rendezvous of the very dregs of society, a/id which were generally known as the Cours des Miraclcs. These houses, ot rather resorts, had been so called, if we are to believe a writer of the early part of the seventeenth century, ' Because rogues . . . and others, who have all day been cripples, maimed, dropsical, and beset with every so n of bodily ailment, come home at night, carrying under theit Arms a sirloin of beef, a joint of veal, or a leg of mutton, not forgetting to hang a bottle of wine to their belts, and, on entering the court, they throw aside rheir crutches, resume their healthy and lusty appearance, and, in imitation of the ancient Bacchanalian revelries, dance all kinds of dances with their trophies in their hands, whilst the host is preparing their suppers. Can there be n greater miracle than is to be seen in this court, where the maimed walk upright? ' 1 In back regions such as these, the very fact that an important effect is not striven for tends to set the tone for interaction, leading those who find them selves there to act as if they were on familiar terms with one another in all matters. cli.it Negro employees can more easily than otherwise be given staff status in American factories if, as in the case of chemists, they can be sequestered from the main regions of factory operation. 'P a u l LaCroix, Manners, Custom, and Dress during the Middle A g es and during the R enaissance Period (London : Chapman and Hall, 1876), p. 471. 76 However, while there is a tendency for a region to become identified as the front region or back region of a performance with which it is regularly associated, still there are many regions which function at one time and in one sense as a front region and at another time and in another se n se as a back region. Thus the private office of an executive is certainly the front region where his status in the organization is intensively expressed by means of the quality of his office furnishings. And yet it is here that he can take his jacket off, loosen his tie, keep a bottle of liquor handy, and act in a chummy and even boisterous way with fellow executives of his own rank. 1 Similarly, of a Sunday morning, a whole household can use the wall around its domestic establishment to conceal a relaxing slovenliness in dress and civil endeavour, extending to all rooms the informality that is usually restricted to the kitchen and bedrooms. So, too, in American m iddle-class neighbourhoods, on afternoons the line between the children’s playground and home may be defined as backstage by mothers, who p ass along it wearing jeans, loafers and a minimum of make*up, a cigarette dangling from their lips as they push their baby carriages and openly talk shop with their colleagues. So also, in working-class quartiers in P aris in the early morning, women feel they have a right to extend the backstage to their circle of neighbouring shops, and they patter down for milk and fresh bread, wearing bedroom slippers, bathrobe, hairnet, and no make-up. And, of course, a region that is thoroughly established as a front region for the regular performance of a particular routine often functions as a back region before and after each performance, for at these times the permanent fixtures may undergo repair, restoration, and rearrangement, or the performers may hold d ress rehearsals. To see this we need only glance into a restaurant, or store, or home, a few minutes before chese establishm ents are opened to us for the day. In general, then, we must keep in mind that when we speak of front and back regions we speak from the reference point of a particular performance, and we speak of the function that the place happens to serve at that time for the given performance. It was suggested that persons who co-operate in staging ' The fact that a small private office can be transformed into a back region by the manageable method of being the only one in it provides one reason why stenographers sometimes prefer to work in a private office as opposed to a large office floor. On a large open floor someone is always likely to be present before whom an impression of industriousness must be maintained; in a small office all pretence of work and decorous behaviour can be dropped when the boss is out. See Richatd Rencke, ' The Status Characteristics of Jobs in a F actory' (Unpublished Master's thesis, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1953), P- 53* 77 the same team-performance tend to be in a familiar relarion to one another. This familiarity tends to be expressed only when the audience is not present, for it conveys an impression of self and team-mate which is ordinarily inconsistent with the impression of self and team-mate one wants to sustain before the audience. Since back regions are typically out of bounds to members of the audience, it is here that we may expect reciprocal familiarity to determine the tone of social intercourse. Similarly, it is in the front region that we may expect a tone of formality to prevail. Throughout our society there tends to be one informal or backstage language of behaviour, and another language of behaviour for occasions when a performance is being presented. The backstage language co n sists of reciprocal first-naming, co-operative decision-making, profanity, open sexual remarks, elaborate griping, smoking, rough informal dress, ' s l oppy' sitting and standing posture, use of dialect or sub-standard speech, mumbling and shouting, playful aggressivity and 'kidding,' inconsiderateness for the other in minor but potentially symbolic acts, minor physical self-involvements such as humming, whistling, chewing, nibbling, belching, and flatulence. The frontstage behaviour language can be taken as the absence (and in some sense the opposite) of this. In general, then, backstage conduct is one which allows minor acts which might easily be taken as symbolic of intimacy and disrespect for others present and for the region, while front region conduct is one which disallows such potentially offensive behaviour. * By invoking a backstage style, individuals can transform any region into a backstage. Thus we find that in many social establishm ents the performers will appropriate a section of the front region and by acting there in a familiar fashion symbolically cut it off from the rest of the region. For instance, in some restaurants in America, especially those called 'one-arm joints,’ the staff will hold court in the booth farthest from the door or clo sest to the kitchen, .inJ there conduct themselves, at least in some respects, as if they were backstage. More important, we ought not to expect that in concrete situations we will find pure examples of informal conduct or 1Ic may be noted (hat backstage behaviour has what psychologists might call a ’ regressive* character. The question, of course, is whether a backstage gives individuals an opportunity ro regress or whether regression, in the clinical sense, is bnckstagc conduct invoked on inappropriate occasions for motives that are not socially approve*!. re formal conduct, although there is usually a tendency to move the definition of the situation in one of these two directions. We will not find rhese pure ca se s because team-mates with respect to one show will he to some degree performers and audience for another show, and performers and audience for one sliow wjil to some extent, however slight, be team-mates with respect to another show. 1’hus in a concrete situation we may expect a predominance of one style or the oclier, with some feelings of guilt or doubt concerning the actual combination or balance that is achieved between the two styles. I would like to em phasize the fact chat activity in a concrete situation is always a compromise between the formal and informal styles by reference to backstage and backstage activity. Three common limitations on backstage informality may be cited. First, when the audience is not present, each member of the team is likely to want to sustain the impression that lie can be trusted with the secrets of the team and that he is not likely to play his part badly when the audience is present. While each team member will want the audience to think of him as a worthy character, he is likely to want his team-mates to think of him as a loyal, well-disciplined performer. Secondly, there are often moments backstage when the performers will have to sustain one another’s morale and maintain the impression that the show that is about to be presented will go over well or that the show that has just been presented did not really go over so badly. Thirdly, if the team contains representatives of fundamental social divisions, such as different age-grades, different ethnic groups, etc., then some discretionary limits will prevail on freedom of backstage activity. Here, no doubt, the most important division is the sexual one, for there seems to be no society in which members of the two sexes, however closely related, do not sustain some appearances before each other. In America, for instance, we learn the following about West Coast shipyards: In iheir ordinary relationships with women workers most of the men were courteous and even gallant. As the women infiltrated the hulls and the remoter shacks of the yard, the men amiably removed their galleries of nudes and pornography from the walls and retired them to the gloom of the tool box. In deference to the presence of 'l a d i e s ,’ manners were improved, faces were shaved more often, and language was toned down. The taboo against improprieties of speech within earshot of women was so extreme a s to be amusing, patticularly since the women themselves frequently gave audible proof that the forbidden words were neither unfamiliar nor disturbing to them. Yet 1 have often seen men who wanted to use strong language, and with good excuse for it, flush with sudden embarrassment and drop their 79 voices to a mutcer on becoming conscious of a feminine audience. In ihe lunchrime companionship of men and women workers and in the casual char at any leisure moment, in all rhar pertained to familiar social contacts, even amid the unfamiliar surroundings of the shipyards» the men preserved almost intact the pattern of behaviour which they practised at home; the respect fot the decent wife and the good mother, the circumspect friendliness with the sister, and even (he )>rotecttvo affection for the inexperienced daughter of the family. 1 Chesterfield makes a similar suggestion about another society: In mixed companies with your equals (for in mixed companies rill people are to a certain degree equal) greater ease and liberty are allowed; but they too have tbeir bounds within bicnscauce. There is a social respect n ecessary; you may start your own subject of conversation with modesty, taking great cate, however, de ne jttmnis parler de conies duns la maison d un pendtu Your words, gestures, and attitudes, have a greater degree of latitude, though by no means an unbounded one. You may have your hands in your pockets, take snuff, sit, stand, or occasionally walk, -as vou lik e; but I believe you would nor think it very bienseanl to whistle, put your hat on, loosen y our garters or your buckles, lie down upon a couch, Or go to bed *ind welter in an -367. 80 of backstage behaviour. We know, of course, that a team with only one member can take a very dark view of itself and that not a few clinical psychologists attempt to alleviate this guilt, making their living by telling individuals the facts of other people’s lives. Behind these realizations about oneself and illusions about others is one of the important dynamics and disappointments of social mobility, be it mobility upward, downward, or sideways. In attempting to escape from a two-faced world of front region and back region behaviour, individuals may feel that in the new position they are attempting to acquire they will be the character projected by individuals in that position and not at the same time a performer. When they arrive, of course, they find their new situation has unanticipated sim ilarities with their old one; both involve a presentation of front to an audience and both involve the presenter in the grubby, gossipy business of staging a show. It is sometimes thought that coarse familiarity is merely a cultural thing, a characteristic, say, of the working cla sse s, and that those of high estate do not conduct themselves in this way. The point, of course, is that persons of high rank tend to operate in small team s and tend to spend much of their day engaged in spoken performances, whereas workingc la s s men tend to be members of large team s and tend to spend much of their day backstage or in unspoken performances. Thus the higher one’s place in the status pyramid, the smaller the number of persons with whom one can be familiar, 1 the le ss time one spends backstage, and the more *An interesting limiting instance occurs in the case of heads of states, who have no team-mates. Sometimes these individuals may make use of a se t of cronies to whom they give a courtesy rank of team-mate when moments of relaxing recreation ate called for. Court equerries often play this role. Ponsonby, op. cit., p. 269, illustrates this in his description of King Edward’s visit to the Danish Court: 'Dinner consisted of several courses and many wines, and usually lasted one and a half hours. M'e then all filed out ami in arm to the drawing-room, where again the King of Denmark and all the Danish Royal Family circled round the room. At eight we retired to our rooms to smoke, but as -the Danish suite accompanied us the conversation was limited to polite enquiries into the customs of the two counrries. At nine we returned to the drawing-room where we played round games, generally Loo, without stakes. 1At ten we were mercifully released and allowed to go to our rooms. T hese evenings were a high trial to everyone, but the King behaved like an angel, playing whist, which was then quite out of date, for very low points. After a week of this, however, he determined to play bridge, but only after the King of Denmark had retited to bed. We went through the usual routine till ten o ’clock, and then Prince Demidoff of the Russian Legation came to the King’s rooms and played bridge with the King, Seymoure Fortescue, and myself, for fairly high points. Ve continued thus till the end of the visit, and it was a pleasure to relax ourselves from the stiffness of the Danish Court.1 81 likely it is that one will be required to be polite as well as decorous. However, when the time and company are right, quite sacred performers will act, and be required to act, in a quite vulgar fashion. i'or numerical and strategic reasons, however, we are likely to learn that labourers use a backstage manner and unlikely to learn that lords use it too. A final point must be suggested about backstage relationships. When we say that persons who co-operate in presenting a performance may express familiarity with one another when not in the presence of the audience, it must be allowed that one can become so habituated to one’s front region activity and front region character that it may be necessary to handle o n e’s relaxation from it as a performance. One may feel obliged, when backstage, to act out of character in a familiar fashion and this can come to be more of a pose than the performance for which it was meant to provide a relaxation. In this chapter I have spoken of the utility of control over backstage and of the dramaturgical trouble that arises when this control cannot be exerted. 1 would like now to consider the problem of controlling ac c e ss to the front region, but in order to do so it will be necessary to extend a little the original frame of reference. Two kinds of bounded regions have been considered: front regions where a particular performance is or may be in progress, and back regions where action occurs that is related to the performance but inconsistent with the appearance fostered by the performance. It would seem reasonable to add a third region, a residual one, namely, all places other than the two already identified. Such a region could be called 'th e outside.' The notion of an outside region that is neither front nor back with respect to a particular performance conforms to our common-sense notion of social establishm ents, for when we look at most buildings we find within them rooms that are regularly or temporarily used as back regions and front regions, and we find that the outer walls of the building cut both types of rooms off from the outside world. Those individuals who are on the outside of the establishment we may call outsiders. While the notion of outside is obvious, unless we handle it with care we will be misled and confused, for when we shift our consideration from the front or back region to the outside we tend also to shift our point of reference from one performance to another. Given a particular ongoing performance as a point of reference, those who are outside will be persons for whom the performers actually or potentially put on a show, 82 but a show (as we shall see) different from, or all too similar to, the one in progress. When outsiders unexpectedly enter the front or the back region of a particular performance-inprogress, the consequence orf their inopportune presence can often best be studied not in terms of its effects upon the performance in progress but rather in terms of its effects upon a different performance, namely, the one which the performers or the audience would ordinarily present before the outsiders at a time and place when the outsiders would be the anticipated audience. In Chapter One of this report it was suggested that performers tend to give the impression, or tend not to contradict the impression, that the role they are playing at the time is their most important role and that the attributes claimed by or imputed to them are their most essential and characteristic attributes. When individuals witness a show that was not meant for them, they may, then, become disillusioned about this show a s well as about the show that w as meant for them. The performer, too, may become confused, as Kenneth Burke su g g ests: We are all, in our compartmentalized responses, like the man who Is a tyrant in his office and a weakling among his family, or like the musician who is assertive in his art and self-effacing in his personal relationships. Such dissociation becomes a difficulty when we attempt to unite these compartments (as, were the man who is a tyrant in his office and a weakling in his home suddenly to employ his wife or children, he would find his dissociative devices inadequate, and might become bewildered and tormented). 1 The answer to this problem is for the performer to segregate his audiences so that the individuals who witness him in one of his roles will not be the individuals who witness him in another of his roles. Thus some French Canadian priests do not want to lead so strict a life that they cannot go swimming at the beach with friends, but they tend to feel that it is best to swim with persons who are not their parishioners, since the familiarity required at the beach is incompatible with the distance and respect required in the parish. Front region control is one measure of audience segregation. Incapacity to maintain this control leaves the performer in a position of not knowing what character he will have to project from one moment to the next, making it difficult for him to effect a dramaturgical su ccess in any one of them. It is not difficult to sympathize with the pharmacist who ac ts like a salesman or like a begrimed stock-man to a customer who proves to have a prescription in her hand, while at the next moment he projects his dignified, disinterested, 'Kenneth Burke. Permanence and Change (New York: New Republic Inc., 1953), fn. p. 309. 83 medical, professionally-spotless pose someone who happens to want a three-cent stamp or a chocolate tudge sundae. 1 It should be clear that just as it is useful for the performer to exclude persons from the audience who see liim in another and inconsistent presentation, so also is it useful for the performer to exclude from the audience those before whom he performed in the past a show' inconsistent with the current one. Persons who are strongly upward or downward mobile accomplish this in a grand manner by making sure to leave rhe place of their origins. And, on the same grounds, just ;is it is convenient to play one's different routines before lifferent persons, so also is it convenient to separate the different audiences one has for the same routine, since that is the only way in which each audience can feel that while there may be other audiences for the same routine, none is getting so desirable a presentation o| it. Here again front region control is important. By proper scheduling of one’s performances, ir is possible not only to keep one’s audiences separated from each other (by appearing before them in different front regions or sequentially in the same region) but also to allow a few moments in between performances so as to extricate oneself psychologically and physically from one’s personal front, while taking on another. Problems sometimes arise, however, in those social establishm ents whc-re the same or dillerent members of the team must handle different audicnces at the same time. If the different audiences come within hearing distance of each other, it will be difficult to sustain the impression that each is receiving special and unique services. Thus, if a hostess wishes to give each of her guests a warm special greeting or farew ell—a special performance, in f a c tthen she will have to arrange to do this in an anteroom that is separated from the room containing the other guests. Similarly, in c a s e s where a firm of undertakers is required to conduct two services on the same day, it will be necessary to route the two audiences through the establishm ent in such a way that their paths will not cross, lest the feeling that the funeral home is a home away from home be destroyed. So, too, in furniture salesrooms, a clerk who is ‘ sw itching’ a customer from one suite of furniture to another of higher price must be careful to keep his audience out of earshot of another clerk who may be switching another customer from a still cheaper suite to the one from which the first clerk is trying to switch his customer, for at such times •See Weinlein, op. cit., pp. 147-148. 84 the suice that one clerk is disparaging will be the su ite that the ocher clerk is praising. 1 Of course, if walls separate the cwo audiences, the performer can sustain the impressions he is fostering by darting rapidly from one region to another. This staging device, possible with two examining rooms, is increasingly popular among American dentists and doctors. When audience segregation fails and an outsider happens upon a performance that was not meant for him, difficult problems in impression management arise. Two accommodative techniques for dealing with these problems may be mentioned. First, all those already in the audience may be suddenly accorded, and accept, temporary backstage sta tu s and collustvely join the performer in abruptly shifting to an act that is a fitting one for the intruder to observe. Thus a husband and wife in the midst of their daily bickering, when suddenly faced with a guest of brief acquaintance, will put aside their intimate quarrels and play out between them selves a relationship that is almost as distant and friendly a s the one played out for the sudden arrival. Relationships, as well as types of conversation, which cannot be shared among the three will be laid aside. In general, then, if the newcomer is to be treated in the manner to which he has become accustomed, the performer must switch rapidly from the performance he was giving to one that the newcomer will feel is proper. Rarely can this be done smoothly enough to preserve the newcomer’s illusion that the show suddenly put on is the performer’s natural show. And even if this is managed, the audience already present is likely to feel that what they had been taking for the performer’s essential self was not so essential. It has been suggested that an intrusion may be handled by having those present switch to a definition of the situation into which the intruder can be incorporated. A second way of handling the problem is to accord the intruder a clear-cut welcome as someone who should have been in the region all along. The same show, more or less, is thus carried on, but it is made to include the newcomer. Thus when an individual pays an unexpected visit to his friends and finds them giving a party, he is usually welcomed loudly and coaxed into staying. If the welcome were not enthusiastically extended, his discovery that he has been excluded might discredit the front of friendliness and affection that obtains between the intruder and his hosts on other occasions. Ordinarily, however, neither of these techniques seem s ‘ See Louse Conant, 'T h e Borax H ouse,’ The American Mercury, XVII, 172. 85 to be very effective. Usually when intruders enter the front region, the performers tend to get ready to begin the performance they stage for the intruders at another time or place, and this sudden readiness to act in a particular way brings at le ast momentary confusion to the line of action the performers are already engaged in. The performers will find themselves temporarily torn between two possible realities, and until signals can be given and received members of the team may have no guide as to what line they are to follow. Embarrassment is almost certain to result. Under such circum stances it is understandable that the intruder may be accorded neither of the accommodative treatments mentioned but rather treated as if he were not there at all or quite unceremoniously asked to stay out. 86 C H A P T E R IV DISCREPANT ROLES One overall objective of any team is to sustain the definition of the situation that its performance fosters. This will involve the over-communication of some facts and the under-communication of others. Given the fragility and the required expressive coherence of the reality that is dramatized by a performance, there are usually facts which, if attention is drawn to them during the performance, would discredit, disrupt, or make useless the impression that the performance fosters. T hese facts may be said to provide 'destructive information.’ A basic problem for many performances, then, is that of information control; the audience must not acquire destructive information about the situation that is being defined for them. In other words, a team must be able to keep its secrets and have its secrets kept. Before proceeding it will be convenient to add some suggestions about types of secrets, because disclosure of different types of secrets can threaten a performance in different ways. The suggested types will be based upon the function the secret performs and the relation of the secret to the conception others have about the possessor; I will assume that any particular secret can represent more than one such type. First, there are what we sometimes call ‘ dark’ secrets. These consist of facts about a team which it knows and conceals and which are incompatible with the image of self that the team attempts to maintain before its audience. Dark secrets are, of course, double se c re ts : one is the crucial fact that is hidden and another is the fact that crucial facts have not been openly admitted. Dark secrets were considered in Chapter One in the section on misrepresentation. Secondly, there are what might be called ’ strate g ic’ secrets. T hese pertain to intentions and capacities of a team which it conceals from its audience in order to prevent them from adapting effectively to the state of affairs the team is planning to bring about. Strategic secrets are the ones that businesses and armies employ in designing future actions against the opposition. So long as a team makes no pretence of being the sort of team that does not have strategic 87 secrets, its strategic secrets need not be dark ones. Yet it is to be noted that even when the strategic secrets of a team are not dark ones, still the disclosure or discovery of such secrets disrupts the team’s performance, for suddenly and unexpectedly the team finds it u se less and foolish to maintain the care, reticence, and studied ambiguity of action that was required prior to lo ss of its secrets. It may be added that secrets that are merely strategic tend to be ones which the team eventually discloses, perforce, when action based upon secret preparations is consummated, whereas an effort may be made to keep dark secrets secret forever. It may also be added that information is often held back not because of its known strategic importance but because it is felt that it may someday acquire such importance. Thirdly, there are what might be called 'in s id e ' secrets. T hese are ones whose possession marks an individual as being a member of a group and helps the group feel separate and different from those individuals who are not 'in the know.’ 1 Inside secrets give objective intellectual content to subjectively felt social distance. Almost all information in a social establishm ent has something of this exclusionary function and may be seen as none of somebody’s business. Inside se crets may have little strategic importance and may not be very dark. When this is :he case, such secrets may be discovered or accidentally disclosed without radically disrupting the team performance; the performers need only shift their secret delight to another matter. Of course, secrets that are strategic and/or dark serve extremely well as inside secrets and we find, in fact, that the strategic and dark character of secrets is often exaggerated for this reason. Interestingly enough, the leaders of a social group are sometimes faced with a dilemma regarding important strategic secrets. Those in the group who are not brought in on the secret will feel excluded and affronted when the secret finally comes to light; on the other hand, the greater the number of persons who are brought in on the secret, the greater the likelihood of intentional or unintentional disclosure. The knowledge that one team can have of another’s secrets provides us with two other types of secrets. First, there are what might be called 'e n tru s te d ’ secrets. This is the kind which the possessor is obliged to keep because of his relation to the team to which the secret refers. If an individual who is entrusted with a secret is to be the person he claim s 1Cf. Riesman's discussion of the ’ inside dopcstex,* op. c U pp. 199*209. 88 he is, 'he must keep the secret, even though it is not a secret about himself. Thus, for example, when a lawyer discloses the improprieties of his clients, two quite different performances are threatened: the clien t’s show of innocence to the court, and the lawyer’s show of trustworthiness to his client. It may also be noted that a team’s strategic secrets, whether dark or not, are likely to be the entrusted secrets of the individual members of the team, for each member of the team is likely to presenc himself to his team-mates as someone who is loyal to the team. The second type of information about another’s secrets may be called ‘ free.’ A free secret is somebody e ls e ’s secret known to oneself that one could disclose without discrediting the image one was presenting of oneself. A team may acquire free secrets by discovery, involuntary disclosure, indiscrcet adm issions, re-transmission, etc. In general we muse see that the free or entrusted secrets of one team may be the dark or strategic secrets of another team, and so a team whose vital secrets are possessed by others will try to oblige the possessors to treat these secrets a s secrets that are entrusted and not free. This chapter is concerned with the kinds of persons who learn about the secrets of a team and with the bases and the threats of their privileged position. Before proceeding, however, it should be made clear that all destructive information is not found in secrets, and that information control involves more than keeping secrets. For example, there seem to be facts about almost every performance which are incompatible with the impression fostered by the performance but which have not been collected and organized into a usable form by anyone. 1 These are in a sense latent secrets, and the problems of keeping secrets are quite different from the problems of keeping latent secrets latent. Another example of destructive information not embodied in secrets is found in such events as unmeant gestures, previously referred to. These events introduce information—a definition of the situation—which is incompatible with the projected claim s of the performers, but these untoward events do not constitute secrets. Avoidance of such expressively inappropriate events is also a kind of information control but will not be considered in th is chapter. 1 1-ot example, Wilensky, op. cit., chap. vii, reports chat a union newspaper may have such low readership that the editor, concerned with his job, ■"ay refuse to have a professional survey made of readership so that »either he nor anyone else will have proof of the suspected ineffectiveness of liis role. 89 Given a particular performance as the point of reference, we have distinguished iliree crucial roles on the basis of function: those who perform; those performed to; arid outsiders who neither perform in the show nor observe it. V/e may also distinguish these crucial roles on the b asis of information ordinarily available to those who play them. Performers are aware of the impression they foster and ordinarily also po ssess destructive information about the show. The audience know what they have been allowed to perceive, qualified by what they can glean unofficially by close observation. In the main, they know the definition of the situation that the performance fosters but do not have destructive information about it. O utsiders know neither the secrets of the performance nor the appearance of reality fostered by it. Finally, the three crucial roles mentioned could be described on the basis of tlie regions to which the role-player has a c c e s s ; performers appear in the front and back regions; the audience appears only in the front region; and the outsiders are excluded from both regions. It is to be noted, then, that during the performance we rpay expect to find correlation among function, information available, and regions of access, so that, for example, if we knew the regions into which an individual ItaJ a c c e s s we should know the role he played and the information he p o ssessed about the performance. In actual fact, however, we find that the congruence among function, information possessed, and ac ce ssib le regions is seldom complete. Additional points of vantage relative to the performance develop which complicate the simple relation among function, information, and place. Some of these peculiar vantage points are so often taken and their significance for the performance comes to be so clearly understood that we can refer to them as roles, although, relative to the three crucial ones, they migfit best be called discrepant roles. Some of the more obvious ones will be considered here. P erhaps the most spectacularly discrepant roles are those which bring a person into a social establishm ent in a false guise. Some varieties may be mentioned. First, there is the role of 'informer.' The informer is someone who pretends to the performers to be a member of their team, is allowed to come backstage and to acquire destructive information, and then openly or secretly se lls out the show to the audience. The political, military, industrial, and criminal variants of this role are famous. If it appears that the individual first joined the team in a sincere way and not with the premeditated plan of disclosing its 90 secrets, we sometimes call him a traitor, turncoat, or quitter, especially if he is the sort of person who ought to have made a decent team-mate. The individual who all along has meant to inform on the team, and originally joins only for this purpose, is sometimes called a spy. It has frequently been noted, of course, that informers, whether traitors or spies, are often in an excellent position to play a double game, selling out the secrets of those who buy secrets from them. Secondly, there is the role of 's h ill.’ A shill is someone who acts a s though he were an ordinary member of the audience but is in fact in league with the performers. Typically, the shill either provides a visible model for the audience of the kind of response the performers are seeking or provides the kind of audience response that is necessary at the moment for the development of the performance. Our appreciation of this role no doubt stem s from fairgrounds, and the designations r s h ill’ and 'c la q u e ,1 employed in the entertainment business, have come into common usage. The following definitions suggest the origins of the concept: Stick, n. An individual—sometimes a local rube—hired by the operator of a set-ioint (a 'f ix e d ' gambling booth) to win flashy prizes so that the crowd will be induced to gamble. When the ’ live o n e s ’ (natives) have been started, the slick s are removed and deliver their winnings to a man outside who has no apparent connection with the joint. 1 jftiltaber, n. An employee of the circus who tushes up to the kid shou ticket box at the psychological moment when the barker concludes his spiel. Me and his fellow shillabers purchase tickets and p ass *nside and the crowd of towners in from of the bally stand are not slow in doing likewise. 2 We must not take the view that shills are found only in non-respectable performances (even though it is only the non-respectable shills, perhaps, who play their role systematically and without personal illusion). For example, at informal conversational gatherings, it is common for a wife to look interested when her husband tells an anecdote and to feed him appropriate leads and cues, although in fact she has heard the anecdote many times and knows that the show her husband is making of telling something for the first time is only a show. A shill, then, is someone who appears to be just another unsophisticated member of the audience and who uses his unapparent sophistication in the interests of the performing team. W'e consider now another impostor in the audience, but (his time one who uses his unapparent sophistication in the interests of the audience, not the performers. T his type ca n be illustrated by the person who is hired to check up on Oavid Mauret, 'C arnival C ant,' Imericnn Speech, VI, 336. 5 VI'. \khite, 'A Circus L ist,' American Speech, 1, 283. 91 the standards that performers maintain in order to ensure that in some respects fostered appearances will not be too far from reality. He acts, officially or unofficially, as a protective agent for the unsuspecting public, playing the role of audience with more perception and ethical strictness rhan ordinary observers are likely to employ. Sometimes these agents play their hands in an open way, giving the performers preliminary warning that the next performance is abouc to be examined. Thus first night performers and arrested persons have fair warning that anything they say will be held a s evidence in judging them. A participant observer who admits his objectives from the beginning gives ihe performers whom he observes a similar opportunity. Sometimes, however, the agent goes underground and by acting as an ordinary gullible member of the audience gives the performers rope with which to hang themselves. In the everyday trades, agents who give no warning are sometimes called 's p o tte rs ,' as they will be here, and are understandably disliked. A salesperson may find that she has been shorttempered and impolite to a customer who is really a company agent checking up on the treatment bona fide customers receive. A grocer may find that he has sold goods at illegal prices to customers who are experts on prices and have authority concerning them. 1 Incidentally, we must be careful to distinguish real spotters from self-appointed ones, often called 'k n o ck e rs' or ' wiseguys,’ who do not po ssess the knowledge of backstage operations that they claim to possess and who are not empowered by law or custom to represent the audience. Today we are accustomed to think of agents who check up on the standards of a performance and on the performers, whether this is done openly or without warning, as part of the service structure, and especially as part of the social control that governmental organizations exert on behalf of the consumer and taxpayer. Frequently, however, this kind of work has been done in a wider social field. Offices of heraldry and offices of protocol provide familiar examples, these agencies serving to keep the nobility and high 1 An illustration a s tegards ctain conductors is given by W. Fred Cottrell, The Railroader (Stanford, California: Stanford University P ress, 1940), p. 87: 'O n ce a train conductor could demand respect of passen g ers; now a 'spotter* may 'turn him in* if he fails to remove his cap as he enters a car where women are seated or does not exude that oily subservience which increasing class consciousness, diffusion of pattern from the European and the hotel world, and the competition with other forms of transportation have forced upon him.’ 92 government officers, and those who falsely claim these statuses, in their proper relative places. There is yet another peculiar fellow in the audience. Me is the one who tak es an unremarked, modest place in the audience and leav es the region when they do, but when he leaves he goes to his employer, a competitor of the team whose performance he lias w itnessed, to report what he has seen. lie is the professional shopper—the Gimbel’s man in Macy's and the Macy’s man in Gimbel’s ; he is the fashion spy and the foreigner at National Air Meets. The shopper i s a person who has a technical right to see the show but ought to have the decency, it is sometimes felt, to stay in his own back region, for his interest in the show is from the wrong perspective, at once more lively and more bored than that of a thoroughly legitimate spectator. Another discrepant role is one that is often called ihe go-between or mediator. The go-between learns the secrets of each side and gives each side the true impression that he will keep its secrets; but he tends to give each side the false impression that he is more loyal to it than to the other side. Sometimes, as in the case of the arbitrator in some labour disputes, the go-between may function as a means by which two obligatorily hostile teams can come to a mutually profitable agreement. Sometimes, as in the ca se of the theatrical agent, the go-between may function as 3 means by which each side is given a slanted version of the other that is calculated to make a closer relationship between the two sides possible. Sometimes, a s in the case of the marriagebroker, the go-between may serve as a means of conveying tentative overtures from one side to the other which, if openly presented, might lead to an embarrassing acceptance or rejection. When a go-between operates in the actual presence of the two team s of which he is a member, we obtain a wonderful display, not unlike a man desperately trying to play tennis with himself. Again we are forced to see that the individual is not the natural unit for our consideration but rather the team and its members. As an individual, the go-between’s activity is bizarre, untenable, and undignified, vacillating as it does from one set of appearances and loyalties to another. As a constituent part of two teams, the go-between’s vacillation is quite understandable. The go-between can be thought of simply as a double-shill. One illustration of the go-between’s role appears in recent studies of the function of the foreman. Not only must he 93 accept the Juries of the director, guiding the show on the factory floor on behalf of the managerial audience, but he must also translate what he knows and what the audience s e e s into a verbal line which his conscience and the audience will be willing to accept. 1 Another illustration of the gobetween’s role is found in the chairman of formally conducted meetings. As soon as he has called the group to order and introduced the guest speaker, he is likely to serve thereafter as a highly visible model for the other listeners, illustrating by exaggerated expressions the involvement and appreciation they ought to be showing, and providing them with advance cues a s to whether a particular remark ought to be greeted by seriousness, laughter, or appreciative chuckles. Speakers tend to accept invitations to speak on the assumption that the chairman will 'ta k e care of them,' which he does by being the very model of a listener and thoroughly confirming the notion that the speech has real significance. The chairman’s performance is effective partly because the listen ers have an obligation to him, an obligation to confirm any definition of the situation which he sponsors, an obligation, in short, to follow the listening-line that he takes. The dramaturgical task of ensuring that the speaker appears to be appreciated and that the listeners are enthralled is of course not easy, and often leaves the chairman in no frame of mind to give thought to what he is ostensibly listening to. The role of go-between seem s to be especially significant in informal convivial interaction, again illustrating the utility of the two-team approach. When one individual in a conversational circle engages in action or speech which receives the concerted attention of the others present, he defines the situation, and he may define it in a way that is not easily acceptable to his audience. Someone present will feel greater responsibility for and to him than the others feel, and we may expect this person clo sest to him to make an effort to translate the differences between speaker and listeners into a view that is more acceptable collectively than the original projection. A moment later, when someone else takes the floor, another individual may find himself taking on the role of go-between and mediator. A spate of informal conversation can, in fact, be seen as the formation and re-formation of teams, and the creation and re-creation of go-betweens. Some discrepant roles have been suggested: the informer, l See Rocchlisbcrger, op. cit. 94 the shill, the spotter, the shopper, and the go-between. In each case we find an unexpected, unapparent relation among feigned role, information possessed, and regions of access. And in each case we deal with someone who may participate in the actual interaction between the performers and audience. A further discrepant role may be considered, that of the ’ nonperson;’ those who play this role are present during the interaction but do not, in a sense, take the role either of performer or of audience, nor do they (as do informers, shills, and spotters) pretend to be what they are not. 1 Perhaps the cla ssic type of non-person in our society is the servant. T his person is expected to be present in the front region while the host is presenting a performance of hospitality to the guests of the establishment. While in some sen ses the servant is part of the host’s team (as I have treated him previously), in certain ways he is defined by both performers and audience as someone who isn’t there. Among some groups, the servant is also expected to enter freely into the back regions, on the theory that no impression need be maintained for him. Mrs Trollope gives us some examples: 1 had, indeed, frequent opportunities of observing this habitual indifference to the presence of their slaves. They calk of them, of their condition, of their faculties, of their conduct, exactly a s if they were incapable of hearing. 1 once saw a young lady, who, when seated at table between a male and a female, was induced by her modesty to intrude on the chair of her female neighbour to avoid the indelicacy of touching the elbow of a man. T once saw this very young lady lacing her stays with the most perfect composure before a negro footman. A Virginian gentleman told me that ever since he had mauied, he had been accustomed to have a negro girl sleep in the same chamber with himself and his wife. I asked for what purpose this nocturnal attendance was necessary? * Good H eaven1” was the reply, "If I wanted a glass of water during the night, what would become of me." 2 T h is is an extreme example. While servants tend to be addressed only when a 're q u e s t’ is to be given them, still their presence in a region typically places some restrictions upon the behaviour of those who are fully present, the more so, apparently, when the social distance between servant and served is not great. In the case of other servant-like roles in our society, such as that of elevator operator and cab-driver, there seems to be uncertainty on both sides of the relationship as to what kind of intimacies are permissible in the presence of the non-person. In addition to those in servant-like roles, there are other standard categories of persons who are sometimes treated 1l’Or a fuller treatment of the role see Goffman, op. cit., chap. xvi. 2 Nirs Trollope, V omestic Manners o f the Americans (2 v o ls .; London: Whittaker, Treacher, 1832), II, 56-57. 95 in their presence as if they were not there; the very young, the very old, and the sick are common examples. Further, we find today a growing body of technical personnel—recording stenographers, 'broadcasting technicians, photographers, secret police, etc.—who play a technical role during important ceremonies but who are not, in a sense, treated as if present. It would seem that the role of non-person usually carried with it some subordination and disrespect, but we must not underestimate the degree to which the person who is given or who tak es such a role can use it as a defence. And it must be added that situations can arise when subordinates find that the only feasible way that they can handle a superordinate is to treat him as if he were not present. Thus, on the island studied by the writer, when the British Public School doctor attended patients in the homes of poor crofters, the residents sometimes handled the difficulty of relating them selves to the doctor by treating him, as best they could, as if he were not present. It may also be added that a team can treat an individual as if he were not present, doing this not because it is the natural thing or the only feasible thing to do, but as a pointed way of expressing hostility to an individual who has conducted himself improperly. In such situations, the important show is to show the outcast that he is being ignored, and the activity that is carried on in order to demonstrate this may itself be of secondary importance. We have considered some types of persons who are not, in a simple sense, performers, audience, or outsiders, 'and who have a c c e s s to information and regions we would not expect of chem. We consider now four additional discrepant roles, involving, in the main, persons who are not present during a performance but who have unexpected information about it. First, there is an important role that might be called 'serv ic e sp ecialist.’ It is filled by individuals who specialize in the construction, repair, and maintenance of the show their clients maintain before other people. Some of these workers, like architects and furniture salesmen, specialize in settings; some, such as dentists, hairdressers, and dermatologists, deal with personal front; some, such as staff economists, accountants, lawyers, and researchers, formulate the factual elem ents of a client’s verbal display, that is, his team’s argument-line or intellectual position. On the basis of concrete research it would seem that service sp ecialists can hardly attend to the needs of an 96 individual performer without acquiring as much, or more destructive information about some asp ec ts of the individual's performance a s the individual himself p o sse sses. Service sp ecialists are like members of the team in that they learn the secrets of the show and obtain a backstage view of it. Unlike members of the team, however, the specialist does not share the risk, the guilt, and the satisfaction of presenting before an audience the show to which he has contributed. And, unlike members of the team, in learning the secrets of others, the others do not learn corresponding secrets about him. It is in this context that we can understand why professional ethics often oblige the specialist to show ‘ discretion,’ i.e., not to give away a show whose secrets h is duties have made him privy to. Thus, for example, psychotherapists who vicariously participate so widely in the domestic warfare of our tim es are pledged to remain silent about what they have learned, except to their supervisors. When the sp ecialist i s of higher general social status than the individuals for whom he provides a service, his general social valuation of them may be confirmed by the particular things he must leam about them. In some situations this becomes a significant factor in maintaining the status quo. Thus in American towns upper-middle c la s s bankers come to see that the owners of.some small businesses present a front for tax purposes that is inconsistent with their banking transactions, and that other businessmen present a confident public front of solvency while privately requesting a loan in an abject, fumbling manner. Middle-class doctors on charity duty who must treat shameful d ise ase s in shameful surroundings are in a similar position, for they make it impossible for a low er-class person to protect himself from the intimate insight of his superordinates. Similarly, a landlord learns that all of his tenants act as if they were the sort who always paid their rent on time but that for some tenants this act is only an act. Persons who are not service specialists are sometimes given the same disillusioning view. In many organizations, for example, an executive officer is required to observe the show of bustling competence that the personnel maintains, although he may secretly p o ssess an accurate and low opinion of some of those who work under him. Sometimes we find, of course, that the general social status of the client is higher than that of the sp ecialists who are retained to attend to his front. In such cases an interesting dilemma of status occurs, with high status and low information control on one side, and low status and high 97 information control on the other. In such ca se s it is possible for the sp e cia list to become overimpressed with the weakn e s se s in the show that his betters put on and to forget the w eaknesses in his own. In consequence, such sp e c ia lists sometimes develop a characteristic ambivalence, feeling cynical about the 'b e tt e r ' world for the sam e reasons that make them vicariously intimate with it. Thus the janitor, by virtue of the service he provides, learns v hat kind of liquor the tenants drink, what kind of food they eat, what letters they receive, what bills they leave unpaid, and whether the lady of the apartment is menstruating behind her uncontaminated front, and how clean the tenants keep the kitcheh, bathroom, and other back regions. 1 Similarly, the American filling station manager is in a position to learn that a man who affects a new Cadillac may buy only a dollar’s worth of gas, or buy a cut-price variety, or seek to wotk the station for free service. And he also knows that the show some men put on of masculine know-how about cars is false, for they can neither diagnose the trouble with their car correctly, ahhough claiming to, nor drive up to the gasoline pumps in a competent way. So, too, persons who sell dresses learn that customers of whom they would not have expected it sometimes have dirty underwear and that customers unabashedly judge a garment by its capacity to misrepresent the facts. Those who sell men’s clothing learn that the gruff show men maintain of being little concerned with how they look is merely a show and that strong, silent men will try on suit after suit, hat after hat, until they appear in the mirror exactly as they want to see themselves. So also, policemen Ieam from the things that reputable businessmen want them to do and not do that the pillars of society have a slight tilt. 2 Hotel maids learn that male guests who make p asses at them upstairs are not quite what the seem liness u* their downstairs conduct suggests. 3 And hotel security officers, or house dicks, as they are more commonly ca li.d , Ieam that a wastebasket may conceal two rejected drafts of a suicide note : Darling— B y the time you gel this I will be it here nothing you ran do will hurt m e liy the time you read this, nothing you con do will be able to h u rt4 l See Ray Gold, *The Chicago Flat Ja n ito r’(Unpublished 'Master's thesis, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1950), especially chap. iv, ‘T he Garbage.’ 2 Wesdey, op. cit., p. 131. 3 Writer’s srudy of an island hotel. 4 Collans, op. cit., p. 156. 98 showing that the final feelings of a desperately uncompromising person were somewhat rehearse:! in order to strike just the right note mid in any case were not final. Service sp ecialists of questionable repute who maintain an office in the back regions of a city so that clients will not be seen seeking assistance clearly provide another example. In Mr Hughes’ words: common scene in fiction depicts a lady of degree seeking, veiled and alone, the address of the fortuneteller or dte midwife of doubtful practice in nn obscure corner of the city. The anonymity of certain sections of cities allows people to seek specialized services, legitimate but embarrassing a s well a s illejtimate, from persons with whom they would not want to be seen by members of their own social circle. • The specialist may, of course, carry his anonymity with him, as does the exterminator who advertises that he will c om e to the client’s house in a van that wears a plain wrapper. Any guarantee of anonymity is, of course, a rather blatant claim that the client has need of it and is willing to make use of it. While it is plain that the specialist whose work requires him to take a backstage view of other people’s performances will be an embarrassment to them, it must be appreciated that by changing the performance which serves as a point of reference other consequences can be seen. We regularly find that clients may retain a specialist not in order to obtain help with a show they are putting on for others but for the very act that is provided by having a specialist attend them— especially if he has a higher general status than his clients. Many women, it seems, go to beauty parlours to be fussed over and called madam and not merely because they need to have their hair done. It has sometimes been claimed, for example, that in Hindu India the procurement of proper service specialists for ritually significant ta sk s is of crucial significance in confirming one’s own caste position. 2 In such ca se s as these, the performer may be interested in being known by the specialist who serves him and not by the show that the service allows him later to perform. And so we find that special sp ecialists arise who fulfil needs that are too shameful for the client to take to sp ecialists before whom he is ordinarily not shameful. Thus the performance that a client stages for his doctor sometimes forces the client to go to a pharmacist for abortives, contraceptives, and venereal disease cures. 3 Similarly, in America, an l E .C . Hughes and Helen M. Hughes, Where People Meet (Glencoe, 111.: Free P re ss, 1952), p. 171, 2 For this and other data on India, and foe suggestions in general, I am indebted to McKim Marriott. •'Veinlein, op. e i t p. 106. 99 individual involved in unseemly entanglements may take his troubles to a Negro lawyer because of the shame he might leel before a white one. 1 It is apparent that service sp ecialists who po ssess entrusted se crets are in a position to exploit their knowledge in order to gain concessions from the performer whose secrets they p ossess. The law, professional ethics, and enlightened self-interest often put a stop to the grosser forms of blackmail, but small concessions delicately requested are frequently unchecked by these forms of social control. P erhaps the tendency to place a lawyer, accountant, economist, or other sp ecialists in verbal fronts on a retainer, and to bring those who are on a retainer into the firm partly represents an effort to ensure discretion; once the verbal specialist becomes |>art of the organization, presumably new methods can be employed to ensure Jbis trustworthiness. By bringing the specialist into one’s organization and even one's team, there is also greater assurance that he will employ his skills in the interests of one’s show and not in the interests of praiseworthy but irrelevant matters such as a balanced view, or the presentation of interesting theoretical data to che specialist's professional audience. 2 A note should be added about one variety of specialist role, the role of 'training sp ecialist.’ Individuals who take this role have the complicated task of teaching the performer how co build up a desirable impression while at the same time taking the part of the future audience and illustrating by punishments the consequences of improprieties. Parents 1 William H. Hale, 'T h e Career Development of the Negro Lawyer' (Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1949), p .72. 2 The specialist in verbal fronts who is brought into the organization will be expected to assem ble and present data in such a way a s to lend maximum support to the claims the team is making at the tin e. The facts of the ca se will ordinarily be an incidcnral matter, merely one ingredient to be considered along wich others, such as the likely arguments of one's opponents, the predisposition of the public ac large to which the team may want to appeal for support, che principles co which everyone concerned will feel obliged to give lip-service, etc. Interestingly enough, the individual who helps collect and formulate the army of faces used in a team’s verbal show may also be employed in che distinctly different task of presenting or conveyiag this tronc in person co the audience. It i s rhe difference between writing the ceremony for a show and performing the ceremony in che show. Here there is a potencial dilemma. The more the sp ecialist can be made to set aside his professional standards and consider only che interests of the team which employs him, the more useful may be che arguments he formulates for them; but the more he has a reputation for being an independent professional, interested only in the balanced facts of the case, che more effeccivc he i s likely to be when he appears before the audience and presencs his finJings. A very rich source of data on these matters is to be found in Wilensky, op. cil. 100 and schoolteachers are perhaps the basic examples of this role in our society; the sergeants who drill officer cadets provide a further example. Performers often feel uneasy in the presence of a trainer whose le sso n s they have long since learned and taken for granted. Trainers tend to evoke for the performer a vivid image of himself that he had repressed, a self-image of someone engaged in the clumsy and embarrassing process of becoming. The performer can make himself forget how foolish he once was, but he cannot make the trainer forget. As Riezler suggests about any shameful fact, *if others know, the fact is established and his image of himself is put beyond his own power of remembering and forgetting.’ 1 Perhaps there is no consistent easy stand that we can take to persons who have seen behind our current front—persons who ‘ knew us w hen’—if at the sam e time they are persons who must symbolize the audience’s response to us and cannot, therefore, be accepted as old team-mates might be. The service specialist has been mentioned as one type of person who is not a performer yet has ac c e ss to backregions and destructive information. A second type is the person who plays the role of 'confidant.' Confidants are persons to whom the performer co n fesses his sins, freely, detailing the sense in which the impression given during a performance was merely an impression. Typically confidants are located outside and participate only vicariously in back and front region activity. It i s to a person of th is kind, for instance, that a husband brings home a daily tale of how he fared in office stratagems,, intrigues, unspoken feelings, and bluffs; and when he writes a letter requesting, resigning from, or accepting a job it is this person who will check through the draft to make sure the letter strikes exactly the right note. And when ex-diplomats and ex-boxers write their memoirs, the reading public is taken behind the scenes and becomes a watered-down confidant of one of the great shows, albeit one that is by then quite over. A person in whom another confides, unlike the service specialist, does not make a business of receiving such confidances; he accepts the information without accepting a fee, as an expression of the friendship, trust, and regard the informant feels for him. We find, however, that clients often attempt to transform their service specialists into confidants (perhaps as a means of ensuring discretion), R ie z le r, op. cit., p. 458. 101 especially when the work of the specialist is merely to listen and talk, as is the c a se with priests and psychotherapists. A third role remains to be considered. Like the role of specialist and confidant, the role of colleague affords those who play ir some information about a performance they do not attend. Colleagues may be defined as persons who present the same routine to the same kind of audience but who do not participate together, as team-mates do, at the same time and place before the same particular audience. Colleagues, >as it is said, share a community of face. In having to put on the same kind of performance, they come to know each other’s difficulties and points of view ; whatever their tongues, they come to speak the same social language. And while colleagues who compete for audiences may keep some strategic se crets from one another, they cannot very well, hide from one another certain things that they hid from the audience. The front that is maintained before others need not be maintained among them selves; relaxation becomes possible. Hughes has recently provided a statement of the com plexeties of this kind of colleague solidarity. Parc of the working code of a position i s discretion*, it allows the colleagues co exchange confidences concerning their relations co ocher people. Among these confidences one finds expressions of cynicism concerning their mission, cheir competence, and the foibles of cheir superiors, them selves, their clients, their subordinates, and the public at large. Such expressions take the burden from one's shoulders and serve a s a defence a s well. The unspoken mutual confidence necessary to them rests on two assumptions concerning one's fellows. The first is that the collegaue will not m isunderstand, the second is that he ■viil not repeat to uniniated ears. To be sure that a new fellow will not misunderstand requires a sparring match of social gestures. The zealot who turns the sparring match into a real battle, who takes a friendly initiation too seriously, is not likely to be trusted with the lighter sort of comment on one's work or with doubts and misgivings; nor can he learn those parts of the working code which arc communicated only by hint and gesture. He is not to be trusted, for, though he is not fit for stratagems, he is suspected of being prone to treason. In order chat men may communicate freely and confidentially they must be able to take a good deal of each other's sentim ents for granted. They must feel easy about their silences a s well a s about their utterances. 1 A good statement of some other asp ects of collegial solidarity is given by Simone de Beauvoir; her intention is to describe the peculiar situation of women, her effect is to tell us about all collegial groups: I he female friendships that she su cceeds in keeping or forming are precious to a woman, but they are very different in kind from relations between men. The latter communicate as individuals through ideas and projects of personal interest, while women are confined within their general feminine lot and bound together by a kind of immanent complicity. And what they look for first of all among themselves 1Hughes and Hughes, K’Aere People Meet, pp. 168-169. 102 is the affirmation of the universe they have in common. They do not d isc u ss opinions and general ideas, hue exchange confidences and recipes; ihcy are in league co create a kind of countcr-universe, the values of which will outweigh masculine values. Collectively they find strength to shake off their ch ain s; they negate the sexual domination of the males by admitting their frigidity to one another, while deriding the men’s desires or their clum sin ess; and diev question ironically the moral and intellectual superiority of their husbands, and of men m g e n e r a l. _ They compare experiences; pregnancies, births, their own and cheir children’s illn esses, and household cares become the essential events of the human story. Their work is not a technique; by passing on recipes for cooking and the like, they endow it with the dignity of a secret science founded on oral tradition. * It should be apparent, tlien, why the terms used to designate one’s colleagues, like the terms used to designate o n e s teammates, come to be in-^group terms, and why terms used co designate audiences tend to be loaded without group sentiment. it is interesting to note that when team-mates come in contact with a stranger who is their colleague, a sort of ceremonial or honorific team membership may be temporarily accorded the newcomer. There is a sort of visiting-fireman complex whereby team-mates treat their visitor a s if he had .suddenly come into very intimate and long-standing relationships with them. Whatever their associational prerogatives, he tends to be given club rights. T hese courtesies are especially given when the visitor and the hosts happen to have received their training in the same establishm ent or from the same trainers, or both. Graduates of the same household, the same professional school, the same penitentiary, the same Public School, or the same small town provide clear examples. When ‘ old b o y s’ meet, it may be difficult to sustain backstage horseplay and the dropping of one’s customary pose may become an obligation and a pose in itself, but it is more difficult to do anything else. An interesting implication of these suggestions is that a team which constantly performs its routines to the same audience may yet be socially more distant from this audience than from a colleague who momentarily comes into contact with the team. Thus the gentry in the island community previously mentioned knew their crofter neighbours very well, having played out the gentry role to them since childhood. Vet a gentry visitor to the island, properly sponsored and introduced, could, in some sen ses, become more intimate with the island gentry in the course of an afternoon tea than could a crofter during a lifetime of contact with his gentry neighbours. It may be suggested that the good will one colleague ceremonially extends to another is perhaps a kind of peace offering: 1'ic Henuvoir. «;». c it., p. M 2. 103 'Y ou don’t cell on us and we won’t tell on you.’ This partly explains why doctors and shopkeepers often give professional courtesies or reductions in price to those who are in some way connected with the trade. We have here a kinvl of bribery of those who are well enough informed to become spotters. The nature of colleagueship allows us to understand something about the important social process of endogamy, whereby a family of one cla ss, caste, occupation, religion, or ethnicity tends to restrict its marriage ties to families of the same status. Persons who are brought together by affinal tie s are brought to a position from which they can see behind each other's front; this is always embarrassing but it is le ss embarrassing if the newcomers backstage have them selves been maintaining the same kind of show and have been privy to the same destructive information. A m isalliance is something that brings backstage and into the team someone who should be kept outside or at least in the audience. It is to be noted that persons who are colleagues in one capacity, and hence on terms of some reciprocal familiarity, may not be colleagues in other respects. It is sometimes felt that a colleague who is in other resp ects a man of lesser power or sta tu s may over-extend his claims of familiarity and threaten the social distance that ought to be maintained on the b asis of these other statuses. In American society, middlec la s s persons of low minority-group sta tu s are often threatened this way by the presumption of their lower-class brethren. As Hughes su g gests in regard to inter-racial colleague relations: The dilemma arises from che face that, while it is bad for the profession to let laymen se e rifts in their ranks, it may be bad for che individual to be associated in the eyes of his actual or potential patients with persons, even colleagues, of so despised a group as the Negro. The favoured way of avoiding the dilemma is co shun contacts with the Negro professional. 1 Similarly, employers who patently have lower-class status, as do some American filling station managers, often find that their employees expect that the whole operation will be conducted in a backstage manner and that commands and directions will be issued only in a pleading or joking fashion. Of course, this kind of threat is increased by the fact that non-colleagues may similarly simplify the situation and judge the individual too much by the collegial company he keeps. But here again we deal with iss u e s that cannot be fully explored unless we change the point of reference from one performance to another. 1Hughes and Hughes, KAere People Meet, p. 172. 104 Just as some persons are thought co ca u se difficulty by n>aking too much of their colleagueship, so others cause trouble by not making enough of it. It is always possible for a disaffected colleague to turn renegade and sell out to the audience the se crets of the act that his onetime brethren are still performing. Every role has its defrocked priests to tell us what goes on in the monastery, and the press has always shown a lively interest in these confessions and expose's. Thus a doctor will describe in print how his colleagues split fees, steal each other’s patients, and specialize in unnecessary operations rhar require the kind of apparatus which gives the patient a dramatic medical show for his money. 1 In Burke’s term, we are thereby supplied with information about the 'rhetoric of medicine.’ J Of course, in a very limited sense, whenever any non-colleague is allowed to become a confidant, someone will have had to be a renegade. Renegades often take a moral stand, saying that it is better to be true to the ideals of the role than to the performers who falsely present them selves in it. A different mode of disaffection occurs when a colleague "goes n a tiv e ’ or becomes a backslider, making no attempt to maintain the kind of front which his authorized status makes or leads his colleagues and the audience to expect of him. Such deviants are said to 'l e t down the sid e.’ Thus in the island community studied by the writer, the inhabitants, in an effort to present them selves a s progressive farmers to visitors from the outside world, felt somewhat hostile to the few crofters who apparently didn’t care and who refused to shave or wash, or construct a front yard, or to supplant the thatched roof of their cottage with something le s s symbolic of traditional peasant status. Similarly, in Chicago there is an organization of blind war veterans who, militant in their desire not to accept a pitiable role, tour the city in order to check up on fellow blind men who let down the side by appealing for alms on street corners. Lewis G. Arrowsmith, 'T h e Young Doctor in New York,’ The American 'lercury, XXII, 1-10. ^Kenneth iJurlcc, A Rhetoric o f Motives (New York: Prentice-llall, 1953). r. 171. Applying this statement to our purposes, we could observe that even the medical equipment of a doctor's office is not to be judged purely for its diagnostic usefulness, hue also hfi3 a function in the rhetoric of niedicine. Uliatevef it is a s apparatus, it also appeals as imagery; and 1f '» man has been treated to a fulsome scries of tappings, sctutinizings, and listenings, with the aid of various scopes, meters, and gauges, he. may feel content to have participated a s a patient in such histrionic actiun, though absolutely no material thing has been done for him, whereas he might count himself cheated if he were given a teal cure, but without the pageantry.’ 105 A final note must be added about colleagueship. There are some colleague groupings whose members are rarely held responsible for each other’s good conduct. Thus mothers are in some resp ects a colleague grouping, and yet ordinarily the misdeeds of one, or her confessions, do not seem to affect closely the respect that is accorded the other members. On the other hand, there are colleague groupings of a more corporate character, whose members are so closely identified in the eyes of other people that to some degree the good reputation of one practitioner depends on the good conduct of the others. If one member is exposed and causes a scandal, then all lose some public repute. As cause and effect of such identification we often find that the members of the grouping are formally organized into a single collectivity which is allowed to represent the professional interests of the grouping and allpweJ to discipline any member who threatens to discredit the definition of the siruation fostered by the other members. Obviously, colleagues of this kind constitute a kind of team, a team that differs from ordinary team s in that the members of its audience are not in immediate face-to-face contact with one another and must communicate their responses to one another at a time when the shows they have seen are no longer before them. Similarly, the collegial renegade is a kind of traitor or turncoat. T he implications of these facts about colleague groupings force us to modify a little the original framework of definitions. We must include a marginal type of 'w e a k 1 audience whose members are not in face-to-face contact with one another during a performance, but who come eventually to pool their responses to the performance they have independently seen. Colleague groupings are not, of course, the only se ts of performers who find an audience of this kind. For example, a department of state or foreign office may lay down the current official line to diplomats who are .scattered throughout the world. In their strict maintenance of this line, and in the intimate co-ordination of the character and timing of their actions, these diplomats obviously function, or are meant to function, a s a single team putting on a single world-wide performance. But of course, in such ca se s, the several members of the audience are not in immediate face-to-face contact with one another. 106 C IIA P T K R V COMMUNICATION OUT OF CHARACTER When two team s present themselves to each other for purposes of interaction, the members of each team tern! to (Maintain the line that they are what they claim to be; they tend to stay in character. Backstage familiarity is suppressed lest the interplay of poses collapse and all the participants find them selves on the same team, as it were, with no one left to play to. Each participant in the interaction ordinarily endeavours to know and keep his place, maintaining whatever balance of formality and informality has been established for the interaction, even to the point of extending this treatment to his own team-mates. At the same time, each team tends to suppress its candid view of itself and of the other team, projecting a conception of self and a conception of other that is relatively acceptable to the other. And to ensure that communication will follow established, narrow channels, each team is prepared to a s sist the other team, tacitly and tactfully, in maintaining the impression it is attempting to foster. Of course, at moments of great crisis, a new se t of motives may suddenly become effective and the established social distance between the team s may sharply increase or decrease, i but when the crisis is past, the previous working consensus is likely to be re-established, albeit bashfully. Underneath and behind this working consensus, and the gentleman’s agreement not to disrupt the interaction upon which this limited consensus is based, there are, typically, le ss apparent currents of communication. If these currents were not undercurrents, if these conceptions were officially communicated instead of communicated in a surreptitious way, they would contradict and discredit the definitions of the situation officially projected by the participants. When 'A n example is found in a recent study of a hospital ward on which experimental treatment was given to volunteers suffering from metabolic disorders about which little was known and for which little could be done. In face of the research demands made upon the patients and the general leeling of ho pelessness about prognosis, the usual sharp line between doctor and patient was blunted. Doctors respectfully consulted with tliuit patients at length about symptoms, and patients came to think of themselves in part as research asso ciates. See Kenee Claire Fox, M ■Sociological Study of Stress : Physician and P atient on a Research Ward,’ Unpublished Ph. L). dissertation, Department of Social Relations, Kadcliff College, 1953. 107 we study a social establishm ent, these discrepant sentim ents are almost always found. They demonstrate that while a performer may act as if h is response in a situation were immediate, unthinking, and spontaneous, and while he himself may think this to be the case, still it will always be possible for situations to arise in which he will convey to one or two persons present the understanding that the show he is maintaining is only and merely a show. The presence, then, of communication out of character provides one argument for the propriety of studying performances in terms of team s and in terms of potential interaction disruptions. It may be repeated that no claim is made that surreptitious communications are any more a reflection of the real reality than are the official communications with which they are inconsistent; the point is that the performer is typically involved in both, and this dual involvement must be carefully managed le st official projections be discredited. Of the many types of communication in which the performer engages and which convey information incompatible with the impression officially maintained during interaction, four types will be considered: treatment of the absent, staging talk, team collusion, and temporary re­ alignments. Treatment of the Absent When th e members of a team go backstage where the audience cannot see or hear them, they very regularly derogate the audience in a way that is inconsistent with the (ace-to-face treatment that is given to the audience. In service trades, for example, custom ers who are treated respectfully during the performance are often ridiculed, gossiped about, caricatured, cursed, and criticized when the performers are backstage; here, too, plans may be worked out for 's e ll in g ’ them, or employing 'a n g l e s ’ against them, or pacifying th e m .1 Similarly, there are very few friendship relationships in which there is not some occasion when attitudes expressed about the friend- behind his back are grossly incompatible with the ones expressed about him to his face. Sometimes, of course, the opposite of derogation occurs, and performers praise their audience in a way that would be impermissible for them to do in the actual presence of the audience. But secret derogation seem s to be much more common than secret praise, perhaps because such derogation serves 1 See, for example, the case report on 'C en tral Haberdashery' in Robert Dubin, ed., Unman Relations in Administration (New York: Prentiec-Mall, 1951) pp. 560*563. 108 to maintain the solidarity of the team, demonstrating mutual regard at the expense of those absent and compensating, perhaps, for the lo s s of self-respect that may occur when the audience must be accorded accommodative face-io-face treatment. Two common techniques of derogating the absent audience may be suggested. First, we often find that when performers are in the region in which they will appear before the audience, and when the audience has left or not yet arrived, the performers will sometimes play out a satire on their interaction with the audience, and with some members of the team taking the role of the audience. Frances Donovan, for example, in describing the sources of fun available to sales-girls, suggests the following: Dot unless they are busy the girls do noc remain long apart. An irresistible attraction draws them together a^ain. At every opportunity they play the game of ‘ customer,* a game which they have invented and of which they never seem to tite—a game which for caricature and comedy, 1 have nevet seen surpassed on any stage. One girl takes the part of the saleswoman, another that of the customer in search of a d ress, and together they put on an act that would delight the heart of a vaudeville audience. 1 A similar situation is described by Dennis Kincaid in his discussion of the kind of social contact that natives arranged for the British during the early part of British rule in India: If the young factors found little pleasure at these entertainments, theit hosts» for all the satisfaction they would at other times have derived from Raji's grace and Kaliani's wit, were too uneasy to enjoy their own party till the guests had gone. Then followed an entertainment of which few English guests were aware. The doors would be shut, and the dancing girls, excellent mimics like all Indians, would give an imitation of the bored guests who had just left, and the uncomfortable tension of the last hour would be dispelled in bursts of happy laughter. And while the English phaetons clattered home Raji and kaliani would be dressed up to caricature English costume ana be executing with indecent exaggeration an Orientalized version of English dances, those minuets and country dances which seemed so innocent and natural to English eyes, so different from the provocative posturing of Indian nautch-girls, but which to Indians appeared utterly scandalous. 2 Among other things, this activity seem s to provide a kind of ritual profanation of the front region a s well a s of the audience, 3 Secondly, we quite regularly find that a consistent difference appears between terms of reference and terms of 1 Frances Donovan, The Saleslady (Chicago: University of Chicago P ress, 1929), p. 39. Specific examples are given on pp. 39-40. 2 Dennis Kincaid, British Social Life in India, 1608-1937 (London: Routledge, 1938), pp. 106-107. 3 A related tendency may be mentioned. In many offices chat are divided into ranked regions, the lunchtime break will find the topmost level leaving the social establishm ent and everyone else in it moving up a region for lunch or for a few moments of afterlunch talk. Momentary possession of the work-place of one's superordinates seem s to offer, among other things, an opportunity to profanize-it in some ways. 109 address. In the presence of the audience, the performers tend to use a favourable form of address to them. This involves, in American society, a politely formal term, such as ' s i r ’ or 'Mr — or a warmly familiar term, such as first name or nickname, the formality or informality being determined by the wishes of the person addressed. In the absence of the audience, the audience tends to be referred to by bare surname, first name where this is not permissible to their faces, nickname, or slighting pronunciation of full name. Sometimes members of the audience are referred to not even by a slighting name hut by a code title which assim ilates them fully to an abstract category. Thus doctors in the absence of a patient may refer to him as ‘ the c a rd ia c ’ or 't h e stre p ;’ barbers privately refer to their custom ers as 'h e a d s of hair.’ So, too, the audience may be referred to in their absence by a collective term combining distance and derogation, suggesting an ingroup-outgroup split. Thus m usicians will call customers squares; native American office girls may secretly refer to their foreign colleagues a s 'G . R.’s ; ’ 1 American soldiers may secretly refer to English soldiers with whom they work a s 'L im e y s ;’ 2 pitchmen in carnivals present their spiel before persons whom they refer to in private, as rubes, natives, or towners; and Jew s act out the routines of the parent society for an audience which is called the goyim. Perhaps the cruelest term of all is found in situations where an individual asks to be called by a familiar term to his face, and this is tolerantly done, but in his absence he is referred to by a formal term. Thus on the island studied by the writer any visitor who asked the local crofters to call him by his first name was sometimes obliged to his face, but in the absence of the visitor a formal term of reference would push him back into what was felt to be his proper place. 1 have suggested two standard ways in which performers derogate their audiences—mock role-playing and uncomplimentary terms of reference. There are other standard ways. For example, when no member of the audience is present, the German Refugees.’ See Gross, op. cil., p. 186. 2Sce Daniel Glaser, 'A Study of Relations between British and American hnlisced Men at ‘SMAHF’,' Unpublished Master's th esis, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago, 1947. Mr Glaser says, p. 16: ‘ T h e term 'lim ey ,’ as used by the Americans in place of 'B ritish, was generally employed with derogatory implications. They would refrain from using it in the presence of the British though the latter usually cither didn’t know what it meant or didn’t give it a derogatory significance. Indeed, the Americans’ care in this respect was rnuch like that of Northern whiles who use the term 'nigger* but refrain from using it in front of a Negro. T his nickname phenomenon is, of course, a common feature of ethnic relations in which categoric contacts prevail.' 110 members of the team may refer to a s p ec ts of their routine in a cynical or purely technical way, giving forceful evidence to them selves that they do not take the same view of their activity as the view they maintain for their audience. A further standard derogation may be cited. When team-mates are warned that the audience is approaching, the team-mates may hold off their performance, purposely, until the very last minute, until the audience almost catches a glimpse of backstage activity. Similarly, the team may race into backstage relaxation the moment the audience has departed. By means of this purposely rapid switch into or out of their act, the team in a se n se can contaminate and profanize the audience by backstage conduct, or rebel against the obligation of maintaining a show before the audience, or make extremely clear the difference between team and audience, and do all of these things without quite being caught out by the audience. Still another standard aggression against those absent occurs in the kidding and ribbing a member of the team receives when he is about to leave (or merely desires to leave) his team-mates and rise or fall or move laterally into the ranks of the audience. At such times the team-mate who is ready to move can be treated a s if he has already moved, and abuse or familiarity can be heaped upon him with impunity, and, by implication, upon the audience. And a final instance of aggression is found when someone from the audience is officially brought into the team. Again, he may be jokingly mistreated and 'given a hard tim e,’ for much the same reason that he was abused when he departed from the team he has just left. 1 The techniques of derogation which have been considered point out the fact that, verbally, individuals are treated relatively well to their faces and relatively badly behind their backs. This seem s to be one of the basic generalizations that can be made about interaction, but we should not seek in our all-too-human nature an explanation of it. As previously suggested, backstage derogation of the audience serves to maintain the morale of the team. And when the audience is present, considerate treatment of them is necessary, not for their sake, or for their sake merely, but so that continuance of peaceful and orderly interaction will be assured. The ‘ ac tu a l’ (eelings of the performers for a member of the audience (.whether positive or negative) seem to have little to do with rhe question, either as a determinant of how this member of the audience is treated to his face or as a determinant of how 1 Cf. Kenneth Burke, A Rhetoric o f Motives, p. 234 ff., who gives a social analysis of the individual being initiated, using as a key word 'h az in g .' H I he is treated behind his back. It may be true that backstage activity often takes the form of a council of war; but when two teams meet on che field of interaction it seem s chat they generally do not meet for peace or for war. They meet under a temporary truce, a working consensus, in order to get their business done. Staging Talk When team-mates are out of the presence of the audience, discussion often turns to problems of staging. Questions are raised about the condition of sign equipment; stands, lines, and positions are tentatively brought forth and 'c le a re d ' by the assembled membership; the merits and demerits of available front regions are analyzed; the size and character of possible audiences for the performance are considered; past performance disruptions and likely disruptions are talked about; news about the teams of one’s colleaguès is transm itted; the reception given one’s last performance is mulled over in what are sometim es called 'p o s t m ortem s;’ wounds are licked and morale is strengthened for the next performance. Staging talk, when called by other names such as gossip, 'sh o p talk,’ etc., is a well-worn notion. I have stressed it here because it helps point up the fact that individuals with widely different social roles live in the same climate of dramaturgical experience. The talks that comedians and scholars give are quite different, but their talk about their work is quite similar. To a surprising degree, before the talk, talkers talk to their friends about what will and will not hold the audience, what will and will not give offence; after the talk, all talkers talk to their friends about the kind of hall they spoke in, the kind of audience they drew, and the kind of reception they obtained. Staging talk has already been referred to in the discussion of backstage activity and collegial solidarity and will not be further discussed here. Team Collusion When a participant conveys something during interaction, we expect him to communicate only through the lips of the character he has chosen to project, openly addressing all of his remarks to the whole interaction so that all persons present are given equal status as recipients of communication. Thus 112 whispering, for example, is often considered improper and is prohibited, for it can destroy the impression that the performer is only what he appears to be and that things are as he has claimed them to .be. 1 In spite of the expectation that everything said by the performer will be in keeping with the definition of the situation fostered by him, he may convey a great deal daring an interaction chat is out of character and convey it in such a way as to prevent the audience as a whole from realizing that anything out of keeping with the definition of the situation has been conveyed. Persons who are admitted to this secret communication are placed in a collusive relationship to one another vis-a-vis the remainder of the participants. By acknowledging to one another that they are keeping relevant secrets from the others present, they acknowledge to one another that the show of candour they maintain, and the show o f being only the characters they officially project, is merely a show. By means of such by-play, performers can affirm a backstage solidarity even while engaged in a performance, expressing with impunity unacceptable things about the audience as well as things about themselves that the audience would find unacceptable. I shall call 'team co llu sio n ’ any collusive communication which is carefully conveyed in such a way as to cause no threat to the illusion that is being fostered for the audience. One important kind of team collusion is found in the system of secret signals through which performers can surreptitiously receive or transmit pertinent information, requests for a s s is tance, and other matters of a kind relevant to the successful presentation of a performance. Typically, these staging cues come from, or to, the director of the performance, and it greatly sim plifies his task of managing impressions to have such a subterranean language available. Staging cues often relate those engaged in presenting a performance to those who are offering assistan ce or direction backstage. Thus, by means of a foot-buzzer, a h o stess can give directions to her kitchen staff while acting as If she is fully involved in the meal-time conversation. Similarly, during radio and television productions a vocabulary of signs is employed by those in the control room l fn recreational games, whispered huddles may be defined as acceptable, a s they may before audiences such as children or foreigners to whom little consideration need be given. In social arrangements in which knots or clusters of persons hold separate conversations in each other’s visible presence, an effort is often made by the participants in each cluster to act as if what they are saying could be said in the other clu ste rs eveïi though it is not. 113 to guide performers, especially a s regards their timing, without allowing the audience to become aware that a system of control communication is in operation in addition to the communication in which performers and audience are officially participating. So also, in b u siness offices, executives who want to terminate interviews both rapidly and tactfully will train their secretaries to interrupt interview s at the proper time with the proper excuse. Another example may be taken from the kind of social establishment in America in which shoes are commonly sold. Sometimes a customer who wants a shoe of larger siz e than the one that is available or the one that fits may be handled as follows: To impress the customer as to the effectiveness of his stretching the shoe, the salesm an may tell the customer chat he is going to strctch the shoes on the thirty-four last. T his phrase tells the wrapper not to stretch the shoes, but to wrap them up as they are and hold them undet the counter for a short while. ‘ Staging cues are, of course, employed between performers and a shill or confederate in the audience, as in the case of ‘ cro ss fire* between a pitchman and his plant among the suckers. More commonly we find th ese cues employed among team-mates while engaged in a performance, these cues in fact providing us with one reason for employing the concept of team instead of analyzing interaction in terms of a pattern of individual performances. This kind of team-mate collusion, for example, plays an important role in impression management in American shops. Clerks in a given store commonly develop their own cues for handling the performance presented to the customer, although certain terms in the vocabulary seem to be relatively standardized and occur in the same form in many shops across the country. When clerks are members of a foreign language group, as is sometimes the case, they may employ this language for secret communication—a practice also employed by parents who spell out words in front of young children and by members of our better cla s s e s who talk to each other in French about things they do not want their children, their domestics, or their tradesmen to hear. However, this tactic, like whispering, is considered crude and impolite; secrets can be kept in this way but not the fact that secrets are being kept. Under such circum stances, team-mates can hardly maintain their front of sincere soliciiude for the customer (or frankness to the children, etc.). Harmless-sounding phrases which the customer thinks he understands are more useful to salespersons. For example, if a customer in a shoe store deeply desires, say, a B width, the salesm an can convince the customer that that is what she is getting: 'D av id Geller, 'Lingo of the Shoe Salesman,' tmerican 'tprerh. I \ , -KV .14 . . . . the salesman will call to another salesman down the aisle and say, "B en n y what size Is this sh o e?'1 By calling the salesman, ' Benny ‘ he implies that the answer should be that the width is B. 1 An engaging illustration of this kind of collusion is given in a paper on the Borax furniture house : Now rhat the customer is in the store, suppose she can ’t be sold? T he price is too high; she must consult her husband; she is only shopping. To let her walk, (i.e., escape without buying) is treason in a Borax House. So an S.O.S. is sent out by the salesman through one of the numerous foot-pushes in the store. In a flash the ’ m anager' is on the scene, preoccupied with a suite and wholly oblivious of the Aladdin who sent for him. "P ardon me, Mr Dixon,” sa y s the salesm an, simulating relucrance in disturbing such a busy personage. "I wonder if you could do something for my customer. She thinks the price of this suite is too high. Madam, this is our manager, Mr Dixon." Mr Dixon d e a r s his throat impressively, lie is all of six feet, has iron-grey hair and wears a Masonic pin on the lapel of his coat- Nobody would susp ect from his appearance that he is only a T.O. man, a special salesman ro whom difficult customers are turned over. " Y e s ," says Mr Dixon, stroking bis well-shaven chin, *1 see. You go on, Bennett. I’ll take care of madam myself. Pm not so busy ■u the moment anyhow.* The salesman slips away, valet-like, though h e ’ll give Dixoo hell if he muffs chat sale. 2 The practice described here of 1T .O .’in g ’ a customer to another salesm an who takes the role of the manager is apparently common in many retail establishm ents. Other illustrations may be taken from a report on the language of furniture salesm en: 'G iv e me the number of this article,’ is a question concerning the price of the article. The forthcoming response is in code. The codc is universal throughout the United States and is conveyed by simply doubling the cost, the salesman knowing what percentage of profit to add on to that. 3 Verlier is used as a command . . . . meaning 'lo s e yourself. 1 It is employed when a salesman wants to let another salesm an know thar the latter's presence is interfering with a sale. 4 In the semi-illegal and high-pressure fringes of our commercial life, it is common to find that team-mates use an explicitly learned vocabulary through which information crucial to the show can be secretly conveyed. Presumably this kind of code is not commonly found in thoroughly respectable circles. 5 We find, however, that team-mates everywhere employ ^David Gelier, op. cit.t p. 284. 2 Conant, op. ctt., p. 174. ■^Charles Miller, ' Furniture Lingo,' American Speech, VI, 128. Albid.%p. 126. 5 An exception, of course, is found in the boss-sccretary relation in respectable establishm ents. Esquire Etiquette, for example, approves the following; p. 24 . Mf you share youc office with your secretary, you will do well to arrange a signal which means you'd like her to get out while you talk to a visitor in private. "Will you leave us alone for a while, MissSmich*” em barrasses everybody; it's easier all around if you can convey the same idea, by prearrangement, with something like, * Will you see if you can settle that business with the merchandising department, Miss Smith?"' 115 an informally and often unconsciously learned vocabulary of gestures and looks by which collusive staging cues can be conveyed. Sometimes these informal cues or 'high s ig n s ' will initiate a phase in a performance. Thus, when ‘ in company,1 a husband may convey to his -wife, by subtle shadings in his tone of voice, or a change in his posture, that the two of them will definitely now start making their farewells. The conjugal team can then maintain an appearance of unity in action which looks spontaneous but often presupposes a strict discipline. Sometimes cues are available by which one performer can warn another that the other is beginning to act out of line. The kick under the table and the narrowed eyes have become humorous examples. A piano accom panist suggests a way by which deviating concert singers can be brought back into tune: He (the accompanist) does this by getting more sharpness into his tone, so that his tone will penetrate to the sing er’s ears, over or rather through his voice. Perhaps one of the notes in the pianoforte harmony is the very note that the singer should be singing, and so he makes this note predominate. When this actual note is not writcen in the pianoforte part, he must add it in the treble clef, where it will pipe loud and clear foe the singer to hear. If the latter is singing a quarter of a tone sharp, or a quarter of a tone flat, it will be an extraordinary feat on his part to continue to sing out of tune, especially if the accompanist plays the vocal line with him for the whole phrase. Once having seen the danger signal the accompanist will continue to be on the qui vive and will sound the singer's note from time to time. 1 The same writer goes on to say something that applies to many kinds of performances: A sensitive singer will need only the most dclicatc of cucs from his partner. Indeed they can be so delicate chat even die singer himself while profiting by them will not be consciously aware of Cnem. The less sensitive the singer, the more pointed and therefore the more obvious these cues will have to be. 2 Another example may be cited from Dale’s discussion of how civil servants during a meeting can cue their minister that he is on treacherous ground: But in the course of conversation new and unforeseen points may well arise. If a civil servant at the committee then se e s his Minister talcing a line which he thinks wrong, he will nor say so flatly; he will either scribble a note to the Minister or he will delicately put forward some fact or suggestion a s a minor modification of h is Minister's view. An experienced Minister will perceive the red light at once and gently withdraw, or at least postpone discussion. It will be clear that the mixture of Ministers and civil servants in a Committee requires on occasion some exercise of tact and some quickness of perception on both sides. 3 Very frequently informal staging cu es will warn team-mates that the audience has suddenly come into their presence; 4 1 Moore, op. cit., pp. 56-57. p. 57. 3 Dale, op. cit., p. 141. A well-known formally-leamed cue of this kind is found in the visual signal 116 or that the coast is clear and thar relaxation of one’s front is now p o ssib le; or that while it may seem all right to drop one’s guard of discretion, there are in fact members of the audience present, making it inadvisable to do so : 1 or that an innocent'ooking member of the audience is really a spotter or shopper or someone who is in other ways more or le ss than he seems. It would be difficult for any team—a family, for example— to manage the im pressions it fosters without such a set of warning signals. A recent memoir concerning a mother and daughter who lived in one room in London provides che following example: On the way p ast Gennaro's 1 became filled wich apprehension about our lunch, wondering how my mother would take to Scotty (a manicurist* colleague she was bringing home to lunch for the first time) and what Scotty would think of my mother, and we were no sooner on the staircase than 1 started to talk in a loud voice to warn her chat 1 was not alone. Indeed, this was quite a signal between us, for when two people live in a single room there is no telling what sort of untidiness can meet the unexpected v isito rs eye. There w as nearly always a cookin§-pan or a dirty plate where it should not be, or stockings or a petticoat drying above the stove. My mother, warned by the raised voice of her ebullient daughter, would rush round like a circus dancer hiding the pan or the plate or the stockings, and then tum herself into a pillar of frozen dignity, very calm, all ready for the visitor. If she had cleared things up too quickly, and forgotten something very obvious, 1 would se e her vigilant eye fixed upon it and I would be expected to do some* thing about it without exciting the visitor's attention. 2 It may be noted, finally, that the more unconsciously these cues are learned and employed, the easier it will be for the members of a team to conceal even from themselves that they do in fact function as a team. As previously suggested, even to its own members, a team may be a secret society. Closely associated with staging cues, we find that teams work out ways of conveying extended verbal m essages to one another in such a way as to protect a projected impression chat might be disrupted were the audience to appreciate that employed in broadcasting studios which lirerally or symbolically reads: ‘ You are on the air.' Another broad gesture is reported by Ponsonby, np, cit , p. 102 : 'T h e Queen (Victoria) often went to sleep during these hot drives, and in order that she should not be seen like this by a crowd in a village, 1 used to dig my spurs into the horse whenever 1 saw a large crowd ahead and make the astonished animal jump about and make a noise. P rin cess [Beatrice alw ays knew that this meant a crowd, and if the Queen didn't wake with the noise 1 made, she woke her herself.' A typical warning cue is illustrated by Katherine Archibald, op, cit., in her study of work in a shipyard : * At times when work was especially slack I have myself stood guard at the door of a tool shack, ready' to warn of the approach of a super* intendent or a front-office boss, while for day after day nine or ten lesser b osses and workmen played poker with passionate absorption.' C r im in a ls commonly employ signals of this kind to warn their colleagues that 'l e g i t ' ears are listening to them or legit eyes are watching them; in criminal argot this warning is called 'giving the office.' 2 Mrs Robert llenrey, Madeleine Grown Up (New York: Dutton, 1953), PP* '16-47. 117 information of this kind was being conveyed. Again we may cite an illustration from the British civil service: It is a very different matter when a civil servant is called on to watch over a Dill in its passage through Parliament, or to go down to either House for a debate. He cannot speak in his own person; he can only supply the Minister with material and suggestions, and hope that he will make good use of them. It need hardly be said that the Minister is carefully *briefed* beforehand for any set speech, as on the second or third reading o f an important Dill, or the introduction of the Department's annual estim ates: for such an occasion the Minister is supplied with full notes on every point likely to be raised, even with anecdotes and Might r e lie f1 of a decorous official nature. He himself, his Private Secretary, and the Permanent Secretary probably spend a good deal of time and labour in selecting from these notes the most effective points to emphasize, arranging them in the best order and devising an impressive peroration. All this is easy both for the Minister and his o fficials; it is done in quiet and at leisure. Dut the crux is the reply at the end of a debate. There the Minister must mainly depend on himself. It is true that the civil servants sitting with patient endurance in the little gallery on the Speaker's right or ac the entrance to the House of Lords, have noted down inaccuracies and distortions of fact, false inferences, misunderstandings of the Government proposals and sim ilar w eak nesses, in the case presented by Opposition speakers: but ir is often difficult co get this ammunition up to the firing-line. Sometimes the Minister’s Parliamentary Private Secretary wili rise from h is seat just behind his chief, stroll carelessly along to the official galiety and hold a whispered conversation with the civil serv an ts: sometimes a note will be p assed along to the Minister: very rarely he himself will come for a moment and ask a question. All these little communications must go on under the eyes of the House, and no Minister cares to seem like an actor who does not know his part ar.d requires to be prompted. 1 B usiness etiquette, perhaps more concerned with strategic secrets than with moral ones, offers the following suggestions: . . . Guard your end of a phone conversation if an outsider is within earshot. If you are taking a message from someone else, and you want to be sure you’ve got it straight, don't repeat the m essage in the usual fashion; instead, ask the caller to repeat it, so your clarion tones won't announce a possibly private m essage to all bystanders. . . . Cover your papers before an outside caller arrives, or make a habit of keeping them in folders or under a covering blank sheer. . . . If you must speak to someone else in your organization when lie is with an outsider, or with anyone who is not concerned with your m essage, do it in such a way chat che chird person doesn ’t pick up any information. You might use the interoffice telephone rather than the intercom, say, or write your m essage on i note you can hand over instead of speaking your piece in public. 2 A visitor who is expected should be announced immediately. If you are closeted with another person your secretary interrupts you to say something like, q Your three o’clock appointment is here. I thought you’d like to know,” (She doesn 't mention the visitor’s name in the hearing of an outsider. If you arc not likely to remember who your ‘ three o’clock appointm ent’ is, she writes the name on a slip of papet and hands it to you, or u ses your private phone instead of the loudspeaker system.) 3 Staging cues have been suggested as one main type of team collusion; another type involves communications which function chiefly to confirm for the performer the fact that he iD ale, op. cit., pp. M8-149. 2 Esquire Etiquette, op. cif., p. 7. E llipsis dots the authors’. * Esquire Etiquette, op. cit., pp. 22-23. 118 does not really hold with the working consensus, thac the show he puts on is only a show, thereby providing himself with at least a private defence against the claim s made by the audience. We may label this activity 'd erisiv e collusion;' it typically involves a secret derogation of the audience although sometimes conceptions of the audience may be conveyed that are too complimentary to fit within the working consensus. We have here a furtive public counterpart of what was described in the seccion called 'Treatm ent of the Absent.’ Derisive collusion occurs most frequently, perhaps, between a performer and hi nisei f. School children provide examples of this when they cross their fingers while telling a lie or stick out their tongues when the teacher momentarily moves to a position where she cannot see the tribute. So, too, employees will often grimace at their boss, or gesticulate a silent curse, performing these a c ts of contempt or insubordination at an angle such that those to whom these acts are directed cannot see them. Perhaps the most timid form of this kind of collusion is found in the practice of 'd o o d lin g 1 or of ‘ going aw ay’ to imaginary pleasant places, while still maintaining some show of performing the part of listener. Derisive collusion also occurs between members of a team when they are presenting a performance. Thus, while a secret code of verbal insults may perhaps be employed only on the lunatic fringe of our commercial life, there i s no commercial establishment so reputable that its clerks do not cast each other knowing looks when in the presence of an undesirable client or a desirable client who conducts himself in an undesirable way. Similarly, in our society it is very difficult for a husband and wife, or two close friends, to spend an evening in convivial interaction with a third person without at some time looking at each other in such a way a s to contradict secretly the attitude they are officially maintaining toward the third person. A more damaging form of this kind of aggression against the audience is found in situations where one member of a team performs his part for the special and secret amusement of his team-mates; for example, he may throw himself into his part with an affective enthusiasm that is at once exaggerated and precise, but so close to what the audience expects that they do not quite realize, or are not sure, that fun is being made of them. 1 A somewhat similar form of collusion occurs ’ Suggested by Howard S. Beckcr in a personal communication. Mr Becker srates rliat jazz musicians obliged to play ’ corny’ music will sometimes play it a little more corny than necessary, the slight exaggeration serving ns a means by which the musicians can convey to each other their contempt for the audience. 119 when one team member attem tps to tease another while both are engaged in a performance. T he immediate object here will be to make one’s team-mate almost burst out laughing, or almost trip, or almost lose his poise in other ways. For example, in the island tourist hotel studied by the writer, the cook would sometimes stand at the kitchen entrance to the front regions of the hotel and solemnly answer with dignity and in standard English the questions put to him by hotel guests, while from within the kitchen the maids, straight-faced, would secretly but persistently goose him. By mocking the the audience or teasing a team-mate, the performer can show not only that he is not bound by the official interaction but also that he has this interaction so much under control that he can toy wirh it at will. A final form of derisive by-play may be mentioned. Often when an individual is interacting with a second individual who is offensive in some way, he will try to catch the eye of a third individual—one who is defined as an outsider to the interaction—and in this way confirm that he is not to be held responsible for the character or behaviour of the second individual. It may be noted in conclusion that all of these forms of derisive collusion tend to arise almost involuntarily, by cues that are conveyed before they can be checked. Reoligning Actions It has been suggested that when individuals come together for the purpose of interaction, each adheres to the part that has been ca st for him within his team’s routine, and each joins with his team-mates in maintaining the appropriate mixture of formality and informality, of distance and intimacy, toward the members of the other team. This does not mean that teammates will openly treat one another in the same way as they openly treat the audience, but it does usually mean that teammates will treat one another differently from the way that would be most 'n a tu r a l1 for them. Collusive communication h as been suggested as one way in which team-mates can free them selves a little from the restrictive requirements of interaction between team s; it is a kind of deviation from type which the audience is meant to remain unaware of, and it tends, therefore, to leave the status quo intact. However, performers rarely seem content with safe channels for expressing discontent with the working consensus. They often attempt to speak out of character in a way that will be heard by the audience but will not openly threaten either the integrity of the two teams or the social 120 distance between them. T hese temporary unofficial, or controlled realignments, often aggressive in character, provide an interesting area for study. When two teams establish an official working consensus as a guarantee for safe social interaction, we may usually detect an unofficial line of communication which each team directs at the other. This unofficial communication may be carried on by innuendo, mimicked accents, well-placed jokes, significant pauses, veiled hints, purposeful kidding, expressive overtones, and many other sign practices. Rules regarding this laxity are quite strict. The communicator has the right to deny that he 'm eant anything1 by his action, should his recipients accuse him to his face of having conveyed something unacceptable, and the recipients have the right to act as if nothing, or only something innocuous, has been conveyed. In many kinds of social interaction, unofficial communication provides a way in which one team can extend a definite but noncompromising invitation to the other, requesting that social distance and formality be increased or decreased, or that both teams shift the interaction to one involving the performance of a new set of roles. This is sometimes known as 'putting out fe e le rs ’ and involves guarded disclosures and hinted demands. By means of statem ents that are carefully ambiguous or that have a secret meaning to the initiate, a performer is able to discover, without dropping his defensive stand, whether or not it is safe to dispense with the current definition of the situation. For example, since it is not necessary to retain social distance or be on guard before those who are one’s colleagues in occupation, ideology, ethnicity, class, etc., it is common for colleagues to develop secret signs which seem innocuous to non-colleagues while at the sam e time they convey to the initiate that he is among his own and can relax the pose he maintains toward the public. Thus the murderous Thugs of nineteenth-century India, who hid their annual depredations behind a nine-month show of civic-minded actions, p o ssessed a code for recognizing one another. As one writer s u g g e s ts : When Thugs meet, though strangers, there is something in their manner which soon discovers itself to each other, and to assure the surmise thus excited, one exclaims 'A le e K han!' which, on being repeated by the other party, a recognition of each other’s habit takes place . . . * Similarly, men of the British working c la ss can be found who still ask a stranger 'how far E a s t’ is he; fellow Freemasons ‘ Col. [.L .S leem an, Tkugs or a Million Murders (London: Sampson Low, n. d.), p. 79. 121 know how to answer this password and know that after they do answ er it those present can relax into intolerance for C atholics and the effete c la sse s. In Anglo-American society the surname and the appearance of persons to whom one is introduced serve a similar function, telling one which of the segments of the population it will be impolitic to cast aspersions against. The guarded disclosure by which two members of an intimate society make them selves known to each other is perhaps the least subtle version of disclosive communication. In everyday life, where individuals have no secret society to disclose their membership in, a more delicate process is involved. When individuals are unfamiliar with each other’s opinions and statu ses, a feeling-out process occurs whereby one individual admits his views or sta tu se s to another a little at a time. After dropping his guard just a little he waits for the other to show reason why it is safe for him to do this, and after this reassurance he can safely drop his guard a little bit more. By phrasing each step in the admission in an ambiguous way, the individual is in a position to halt the procedure of dropping his front at the point where he gets no confirmation from the other, and at this point he can act as if h is la st disclosure were not an overture at all. Thus when two persons in conversation are attempting to discover how careful they are going to have to be about stating their true political opinions, one of them can halt his gradual disclosure of how far left or how far right he is just a t the point where the other has come to the furthest extreme of his actual beliefs. In such cases, the person with the more extreme views will tactfully act as if his views are no more extreme than the other’s. T his process of gradual guarded disclosure is also illustrated by some of the mythology and a few of the facts associated with sexual life in our society. The sexual relation is defined as one of intimacy with initiative superordination for the male. In fact, courting practices involve a concerted aggression against the alignment between the sexes on the part of the male, as he attempts to manoeuvre someone for whom he must at first show respect into a position of subordinate intimacy. However, an even more aggressive action against the alignment between the sexes is found in situations where the working consensus is defined in terms of superordination and distance on the part of a performer who happens to be a woman and subordination on the part of a performer who happens to be a man. The possibility arises that the 122 male performer will redefine the sicuation to emphasize his sexual superordination as opposed to his socio-economic subordination. 1 In our proletarian literature, for example, it is the poor man who introduces th is redefinition in regard to a rich woman; Lady Chatterley's J^over, as has often been remarked, is a clear-cut example. And when we study service occupations, especially lowly ones, inevitably we find that practitioners have anecdotes to tell about the time they or one of their colleagues redefined the service relacion inco a sexual one (or had it redefined for them). T ales of such aggressive redefinitions are a significant part of the mythology not only of particular occupations but also of the male subculture generally. Temporary realignments through which iirection of the interaction may be seized in an unofficial way by a subordinate, or unofficially extended by a superordinate, attain some kind of stability and insticutionalization in what is sometimes called ' double-talk.’ 2 By this communication technique two individuals may convey information to one another in a manner or on a matter chat is inconsistent with their official relationship. Double-talk involves the kind of innuendo that can be conveyed by both sides and carried on for a sustained period of time. It is a kind of collusive communication differenc from other types of collusion in thac the characters against whom the collusion is sustained are projecced by the very persons who enter into the collusion. Typically double-talk occurs during interaction between a subordinate and a superordinate concerning matters which are officially outside the the competence and jurisdiction of the subordinate buc which actually depend on him. By employing double-talk the subordinate can initiate lines of action without giving open recognition to the expressive implication of such initiation and without putting into jeopardy the statu s difference between himself and his superordinate. Barracks and jails apparently abound in double-talk. It is also commonly found in situations ’ P erhaps because of respect for the Freudian ethic, some sociologists seem to a c t a s if it would be io bad taste, impious, or self-revelatory to define sexual intercourse a s part of the ceremonial system, a reciprocal ritual performed to confirm symbolically an exclusive social relationship. T h is chapter draws heavily on Kenneth Burke, who clearly takes the sociological view in defining courtship as a principle of rhetoric through which social estrangements are transcended. See Burke, A Grammar o f Motives, p. 208 ff. and pp. 267-268. 2In everyday speech the term 'd o u b le-talk ' is also used in two other senses : it i s used to refer to sentences in which sounds have been injected which seem a s if they mi^ht be meaningful but really are not; it is used to refer to protectively ambiguous answers to questions for which the asker desired a c le a r c u t reply. 123 where the subordinate has had long experience with the job whereas the superordinate has not, as in the split which occurs in government offices between a 'perm anent’ deputy minister and a politically appointed minister, and in those c a se s where the subordinate speaks the language of a group of employees but his superordinate does not. We may also find double-talk in situations where two persons engage in illicit agreements with ‘each other, for by this technique communication may occur and yet neither participant need place himself in the hands of the other. A similar form of collusion is sometimes found between two team s which must maintain the impression of being relatively hostile or relatively distant toward each other and yet find it mutually profitable to come to an agreement on certain matters, providing this does not em barrass the oppositional stand they are obliged to be ready to maintain toward each other. 1 In other words, deals can be made without creating the mutual-solidarity relationship which dealing usually lead s to. More important, perhaps, double-talk regularly occurs in intimate domestic and work situations, as a safe means of making and refusing requests and commands that could not be openly made or openly refused without altering the relationship. I have considered some common realigning actions—movements around, or over, or away from the line between the team s; processes such a s "nofficial grumbling, guarded disclosures, and double-talk were given as instances. 1 would like to add a few more types to the picture. When the working consensus established between two teams is one involving avowed opposition, we find that the division of labour within each team may ultimately lead to momentary realignments of the kind that make us appreciate that not only armies have the problem of fraternization. A specialist on one team may find that he has a great deal in common with his opposite number on the other team and that together they talk a language which tends to align them together on a single team in opposition to all the remaining participants. Thus, during labour-management negotiations, opposing lawyers may find them selves exchanging collusive looks when a layman on either team makes a patent legal gaffe. When the sp ecialists are not permanently part of a particular team but rather hire them selves out for the duration of negotiations, they are likely to be more loyal in some se n se to their catling and their l See Dale, op. cit., pp. 182-183, foe an illustration of tacit compromises between two teams officially opposed to each other. See also Melville Dalton, 'U nofficial Uaion-Management Relations,’ American Sociological R eview , XV, pp. 611-619. 124 colleagues chan to rhe team they happen at the time to be serving. If, then, the impression of opposition between che team s is to be maintained, the cross-cutting loyalties of sp e cia lists will have to be suppressed or expressed surreptitiously. Thus American lawyers, in sensing that their clients want them to be hostile to the opposing lawyer, may wait until a backstage rec ess before having a friendly collegial chat about the ca se in progress. In discu ssin g the role that civil servants play in parliamentary debates, Dale makes a similar suggestion: A sec debate on one subject . . . . as a rule takes only one day. If a Department is so unlucky a s to have a long and contentious BUI in Committee of the whole House, the Minister and the civil servants in charge of it must be there from 4 p.m. till 11 p.m. (sometimes much later if the 1! o' clock rule is suspended), perhaps day after day from Monday till Thursday every w e e k . . . . However, the civil servants get one compensation for their sufferings. It is at this time that they are most likely to renew and extend their acquaintances in the House. The sense of pressure is less both amon£ Members and among officials than during a set debate of one day : it is legitimate to escap e ftom the debating chamber to the smoking*room ot the terrace and engage in cheerful conversation while a notorious bote is moving an amendment which everyone knows to be impossible. A certain camaraderie arises among all engaged night after night upon a Bill, Government Opposition, and civil servants alike. 1 Interestingly enough, in some c a se s even backstage fraternization may be considered too much of a threat to the show. Thus baseball players whose teams will represent opposing sid e s of fans are required by league ruling to refrain from convivial conversation with one another just before the game starts. T his is a readily understandable rule. It would not be seemly to se e players chinning as if they were at an afternoon tea, and then hope to support the point chac they go after each other hell-bent for leather, which they do, as soon as the game begins. They have to act like opponents all the time. 2 In all of these ca se s involving fraternization between opposing specia lists, the point is not that the secrets of the teams will be disclosed or their interests made to suffer (although this may occur and may appear to occur) but rather that the impression of opposition that is fostered between the teams may be discredited. The contribution of the specialist must appear to be a spontaneous response to the facts of the case, independently placing him in opposition to the other team ; when he fraternizes with his opposite number the technical value of his contribution may not suffer, but, dramaturgically speaking, it is shown up for what it is —the purchased performance of a routine task. I do not mean to imply by this discussion that fraternization 1 UUe, op. cit., p. 150. -Pinelli, op cif., p. 169 125 occurs only between sp e cia lists temporarily taking sides against each other. Whenever loyalties cross-cut, a set of individuals may loudly form one pair of team s while quietly forming another. Often, when two team s enter social interaction, we can identify one as having the lower general prestige and the other team the higher. Ordinarily, when we think of realigning actions in such ca se s, we think of efforts on the part of the lower team to alter the basis of interaction in a direction more favourable to them or to decrease the social distance and formality between them selves and the higher team. Interestingly enough, there are occasions when it serves the wider goals of the higher team to lower barriers and admit the lower team to greater intimacy and equality with it. Granting the consequences of extending backstage familiarity to one’s lessers, it may be in one’s long-range interest to do so momentarily. Thus, in order to prevent a strike, Mr Barnard tells us he deliberately swore in the presence of a committee representing unemployed workers and also tells us that he is aware of the significance of th is : In my judgment, confirmed by others whose opinion I respect, it is as a general rule exceedingly bad practice fot one In a superior position to sw ear at or in the presence of those of subordinate or inferior status» even though the latter have no objection to oaths and even though they know the superior is accustomed to cursing. I have known very few men who could do it without adyerse reactions on rhelr influence. I suppose the reason is that whatever lowers the dignity of a superior position makes it more difficult to accept difference of position. Also, where a single organization is involved in which the superior position is symbolic of the whole organization, the prestige of the latter is thought to be injured. In the present case, an exception, the oath was deliberate and accompanied by hard pounding of the table. 1 A sim ilar situation is found in those mental hospitals where milieu therapy is practised. By bringing the nurse and even attendants into what are usually sacrosanct staff conferences, these non-medical staff persons can feel that the d istan ce between them selves and the doctors is decreasing and may show more read in ess to take the doctors’ point of view toward the patients. By sacrificing the ex clusiveness of those at the top, it is felt that the morale of those at the bottom can be increased. A staid report of this process is given us by Maxwell Jones in his report on English experience with milieu therapy: 1 Chester I. Barnard, Organization and Management (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University P re ss , 1949), n. pp. 73-74. T his kind of conduct must be clearly distinguished from the rough language and behaviour employed by a superordioate who sta y s within the team made up of his employees and 'k i d s ' them into work. 126 In che unit we have attempted co develop the role of the doctor to meer our limited treatment goat and have tried to avoid pteience. T h is has meant a considerable break from hospital tradition. Vic do not dress to conform co the usual concept of the professional man. Vt'e have avoided the white coat, prominent stethoscope, and aggressive percussion hammer a s excensions of our body image. 1 Actually, when we study the interaction between two teams in everyday situations we find that often the superordinate team will be expected to unbend just a little. For one thing, such relaxation of front provides a basis for barter; the superordinate receives a service or good of some kind, while the subordinate receives an indulgent grant of intimacy. Thus, the reserve which upper-class people in Britain maintain during interaction with tradesmen and petty officials has been known to give way momentarily when a particular favour must be asked of these subordinates. Also, such relaxation of d istance provides one means by which a feeling of spontaneity and involvemenc can be generated in the interaction. In any case, interaction between two teams often involves the taking of very small liberties, if only a s a means of testing the ground to see if unexpected advantage might not be taken of the opposing side. When a performer refuses to keep his place, whether it is of higher or lower rank than the audience, we may expect that the director, if there is one, and the audience may well become ill-disposed toward him. In many cases, the rank and file are also likely to object to him. As previously suggested in reference to ratebusters, any extra concession to the audience on the part of one member of the team is a threat to the stand the others have taken and a threat to the security they obtain from knowing and controlling the stand they will have to take. Thus, when one teacher in a school is deeply sympathetic to her charges, or enters into their play during recess, or is willing to come into close contact with the low -status ones among them, the other teachers will find that the impression they are trying to maintain of what constitutes appropriate work is threatened. 1 In fact, when particular performers cross the line that separates the teams, when someone becomes too intimate, or too indulgent, or too antagonistic, we may expect a circuit of reverberations to be set up which affects the subordinate team, the superordinate team, and the particular transgressors. ___________ 1 Maxwell Jones, The Therapeutic Community (New York: Basic Books, 1953), p. 40. 2 Personal communication ftom Helen Biaw, schoolteacher. 127 A hint of such reverberations may be cited from a recent study of merchant seamen, in which the author suggests that when officers quarrel in matters regarding ship duty, the seamen will avail them selves of the breach by offering their commiserations to the officer chey feel has been wronged: In doing th is (playing up to one of the disputants) the crewmen expected the officer to relax in his superior attitude and to allow the men a certain equality while d isc u ssin g the situation. T h is soon led to their expecting certain privileges—sucn a s standing in the wheel-house instead of on the wings of the bridge. They took advantage o f the m ates' dispute to ease their subordinate statu s. 1 Recenc trends in psychiatric treatment provide us with other exam ples; I would like to mention some of these. One instance may be taken from the Maxwell Jo n e s’ report, although his study purports to be an argumenc for easing sta tu s differences between staff levels and between patients and staff: The integrity of the n u rses’ group can be upset by the indiscretion of any one member: a nutse who allow s her sexual needs to be met in an overt way by the patient alters the patien t’s attitude towards the whole nursing group and makes the nurse's therapeutic role a less effective one. 2 Another illustration is found in Bettelheim ’s comments on his experience in constructing a therapeutic milieu at the Sonia Shankman Orthogenic School at the University of Chicago: Within the total setting of the therapeutic milieu, personal security, adequate instinctual gratification and group support all sensitize the child to incer-personal relations. It would, of course, defeat the putposes of milieu therapy if the children were not also safeguarded from the kind of disillusionm ent they have already experienced in their original settings. Staff coherency is therefore an important source of personal security to the children a s the staff members remain impervious to the children’s attempts to play off one staff member against another. Originally, many children win the affection of one parent only at the co st of affectionate claims on the other. A child’s means of controlling the family sicuarion by pitting one parent against the other is often developed on this basis, but gives him no more than a relative security. Children who have used this technique with particular su c c e ss are especially handicapped in their ability to form unambivalent relationships later on. In any case, a s the children recreate oedipal situations in the school they also form positive, negative or ambivalent attachm ents to various staff members. It is essential that th ese relationships between children and individual staff members do not affect the relationships of staff members to each other. Without coherence in this area of the total milieu such attachm ents might deteriorate into neurotic relationships and destroy the basis of identification and sustained affectionate attachm ents. 3 A final illustration may be taken from a group therapy project, in which suggestions are sketched in for handling recurrent interaction difficulties caused by troublesome patien ts: Attempts are made to establish a special relationship with the doctor. P atien ts often attempt to cultivate the illusion of a secret understanding with the doctor by, for example, trying to catch h is eye ■Beattie, op. cil., pp. 25-26. 2Maxwell Jones, op. ciU, p. 38. 3 Bruno Betrelheim and Emmy Sylvester, ‘ Milieu T herapy,’ Psychoanalytic Review, XXXVI, 65. 128 if one patient brings up something that sounds 'c ra z y .' If they succeed in getting a response from the doctor which they can interpret as indicating a special bond, it can be very disrupting to the group. Since this type of dangerous by-play is characteristically non-verbal, the doctor must especially control his own non-verbal activity. 1 Perhaps these citations tell us more about the parriy hidden social sentiment? of the writers than about the general processes that can occur when someone steps out of line, but recently, in the work of Stanton and Schwartz, we have been given a fairly detailed report of the circuit of consequences which arises when the line between two teams is crossed. 2 It was suggested that at times of crisis lines may momentarily break and members of opposing teams may momentarily forget their appropriate pla ces with respect to one another. It was also suggested that certain purposes can sometimes be served, apparently, when barriers between teams are lowered, and . that to achieve these purposes superordinate teams may temporarily join with the lower ranks. It must be added, as a kind of limiting case, that interacting teams sometimes seem to be prepared to step out of the dramatic framework for their actions and give them selves up for extended periods of time to a promiscuous orgy of clinical, religious, or ethical analysis. We can find a lurid version of this process in evangelical social movements which employ the open confession. A sinner, sometimes admittedly not of very high status, stands up and tells to those who are present things he would ordinarily attempt to conceal or rationalize away; he sacrifices his secrets and his self-protective distance from others, and this sacrifice tends to induce a backstage solidarity among all present. Group therapy affords a similar mechanism for the building up of team spirit and backstage solidarity. A psychic sinnet 1 Florence B. Powdermaker and others, 'Prelim inary Report for the National Research Council: Group Thetapy Research P ro je c t,’ p. 26. (This research has since been reported by Powdermaker and' Jerome D. Frank, Group Psychotherapy (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University P ress, 1953).) Defrayal of one's team by catching the eye of a member of the other team is, of course, a common occurence. It may fee noted that in everyday life refusal to enter into momentary collusive communication of this kind when one has been invired to do so is itself a minor affront ro the inviter. One may find oneseLf in a dilemma as to whether to betray the object of the requested collusion or to affront the person requesting the collusion. An example is provided by Ivy Compton-Burnett, A Family and a Fortune (London: Eyre & Sp oeti swoode, 1948), p. 13* " B u t 1 was not snoring," said Blanche, in the easier tone of losing grasp of a situation. * 1 should have known it myself. It would not be possible to be awake and m ake a noise and not hear it. 71 ’ Ju stin e gave an arch look at anyone who would receive it. Edgar did so a s a duty and rapidly withdrew h is eyes a s another. 1 2 Alfred H. Stanton and Morris S. Schwartz, 'T h e Management of a Type of Institutional Participation in Mental Illn e ss,1 Psychiatry, XII, 13-26. in this paper the writers describe nurse-sponsorship of particular patients in terms of its effects upon other patients, the staff, and the transgressors. 129 stands up and talks about himself and invites others to talk about him in a way that would be im possible in ordinary interaction. Ingroup solidarity tends to result, and this 's o c ia l support,’ a s it is called, presumably h as therapeutic value. (By everyday standards, the only thing a patient lo se s in this way is his self-respect.) Perhaps an echo of this is also to be found in the nurse-doctor meetings previously mentioned. It may be that these shifts from apartness to intimacy occur at times of chronic strain. Or perhaps we can view them as part of an anti-dramaturgical social movement, a cult of confession. Perhaps such lowering of barriers represents a natural phase in the social change which transforms one team into another: presumably opposing teams trade secrets so that they can start at the beginning to collect a new set of skeletons for a newly shared closet. In any case, we find that occasions arise when opposing teams, be they industrial, marital, national, etc., seem ready not only to tell their secrets to the same sp ecialist but also to perform th is disclosure in the enemy’s presence. 1 In conclusion I would like to suggest that one of the most fruitful places to study realigning actions, especially temporary betrayals, may not be in hierarchically organized establishm ents but during informal convivial interaction among relative equals. In fact, the sanctioned occurrence of these aggressions seem s to be one of the defining characteristics of our convivial life. It is often expected on such occasions that two persons will engage each other in a sparring conversation for the benefit of listen ers and that each will attempt, in an unserious way, to discredit the position taken by the other. Flirting may occur in which males will try to destroy the females’ pose of virginal unapproachability, while females may attempt to force from males a commitment of concern without at the same time weakening their own defensive position. (Where those who flirt are at the same time members of different connubial teams, relatively unserious betrayals and sell-outs may also occur.) In conversational circles of five or six, basic alignments as between one conjugal pair and another, or between hosts and guests, or between men and women, may be lightheartedly set aside, and the participants will stand ready to shift and reshift team alignments with little provocation, jokingly joining their previous audience against their previous 'An example may be seen in the claimed role of the Tavistock group as th erap ists for ’ working through' the antagonism of labour and management in industrial establishm ents. See the consultation records reported in E liot Jaques, The Changing Culture of a Factory (London: Tavistock Ltd., 1951). 130 team-mates by means of open betrayal of them or by mock collusive communication against them. It may also be defined a s fitting if someone present of high sta tu s be made drunk and made to drop his front and become intimately approachable by h is somewhaC-lessers. T he same aggressive tone is often achieved in a le s s sophisticated way by playing games or jokes in which the person who i s the butt will be led unseriously, into taking a position that is ludicrously untenable.’ QJOOOUJXUÜ In this chapter I have considered four types of communication out of character: treatment of the ab sen t; staging talk; team collusion; and realigning actions. Each of th e se four types o f conduct directs attention to the sam e point: the performance given by a team is not a spontaneous, immediate response to the situation, absorbing all of the team’s energies and constituting their sole social reality; the performance is something the team members can stand back from, back far enough to imagine or play out simultaneously other kinds of performances attesting to other realities. Whether th e performers feel their official offering is the ‘ re a lis t’ reality or not, they will give surreptitious expression to multiple versions of reality, each tending to be incompatible with the others. 131 C H A P T E R VI THE ARTS OF IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT In this chapter I would like to bring together what has been said or implied about the attributes that are required of a performer for the work of successfully staging a character, by referring briefly to some of the techniques of impression management in which these attributes are expressed. As an introduction to this attempt, it may be well to suggest, in some c a s e s for the second time, some of the principal types of performance disruptions, for it is these disruptions which the techniques of impression management function to avoid. In the beginning of this report, in considering the general ch aracteristics of performances, it was suggested that the performer must act with expressive responsibility, since many minor, inadvertent a c ts happen to be well designed to convey im pressions inappropriate at the time. T hese events were called 'unm eant g estu res.’ Ponsonby gives an illustration of how a director’s attempt to avoid an unmeant gesture led to the occurrence of another. One of the Attachés from the Legation was to c a n y the cushion on which the insignia were placed, and in otder to prevent their falling off I stuck the pin at the back of the Star through the velvet cushion. The Attaché', howevet, was not content with this, but sccured the end of th e pin by the catch to make doubly sure. The result was that when Prince Alexander, having made a suitable spcech, tried to get hold of the Star, he found it firmly fixed to the cushion and spent some rime in getting it loose. T his rather .spoilt the most impressive moment of the ccrcmony. 1 It should be added that the individual held responsible for contributing an unmeant gesture may chiefly discredit his own performance by this, a team-mate’s performance, or the performance being staged by his audience. When an outsider accidentally enters a region in which a performance is being given, or when a member of the audience inadvertently enters the backstage, the intruder is likely to catch those present flagrante delicto. Through no one’s intention, the persons present in the region may find that they have patently been witnessed in activity that is quite incompatible with the impression that they are, for wider social reasons, in a position to maintain to the intruder. We deal here with what are sometim es called 'inopportune intrusions.’ 'P onsonby, op. eft., p. 351. 132 The past rife and current round of activity of a given performer typically contain at le a st a few facts which, if introduced during the performance, would discredit or at le ast weaken the claim s about self that the performer w as attempting io project as part of the definition of the situation. T h ese facts may involve well-kept dark secrets or negatively-valued ch aracteristics that everyone can see but no one refers to. When such facts are introduced, em barrassment is the usual result. T hese facts can, of course, be brought to one’s attention by unmeant g estures or inopporcune intrusions. However, they are more frequently introduced by intentional verbal statem ents c r non-verbal a c ts whose full significance is not appreciated by :he individual who contributes them co the interaction. Following common usage, such disruptions of projections may be called 'fa u x p a s .’ Where a performer unthinkingly makes ari incentional contribution which destroys his own team’s inv-'.£e we may speak of ‘ g a ffe s ’ or : boners.’ Where a performer jecps-fdizes the image of self projected by the other team, we may speak of 'b r i c k s ’ or of the performer having 'p u t his foot in it.' : Unmeant gestures, inopporcune intrusions, and faux Das are sources of em barrassm ent and dissonance which are typically unintended by the- person who is responsible for making them and which woa.lo be avoided were the individual tc ‘( their team. T his sort of thing is em barrassing e'nough, but when the unguarded request is refused to the individual’s face, he suffers what we can call humiliation. We have considered some major forms of performance disruption—unmeant gestures, inopportune intrusions, faux pas, and scen es. T h ese disruptions, in everyday term s, are 134 often called 'in c id e n ts .’ When an incident occurs, the reality sponsored by the performers is threatened. The persons present are likely to react by becoming flustered, ill at ease, embarrassed, nervous, and the like. Quite literally, the participants may find them selves out of countenance. When these flusterings or symptoms of em barrassm ent become perceived, the reality that is supported by the performance is likely to be further jeopardized and weakened, for th ese signs of nervousness in most c a s e s are an asp ec t of the individual who p resen ts a character and not an aspect of the character he projects, thus forcing upon the audience an image of the man behind the mask. In order to prevent the occurrence of incidents and the embarrassment consequent upon them, it will be necessary for all the particip an ts in the interaction, a s well a s those who do not participate, to p o s s e s s certain attributes and to express th ese attributes in practices employed for saving the show. T h e se attributes and p rac tices will be reviewed under three headings: the defensive m easures used by performers to save their own show; the protective m easures used by audience and outsiders to a s s is t the performers in saving the performers’ show ; and, finally, the m easures the performers m ust take ill order to make it p o ssib le for the audience and outsiders to employ protective m easures on the performers’ behalf. Defensive Attributes and Practices 1. DRAMATURGICAL LOYALTY. It is apparent that if a team is to sustain the line it h as taken, the team-mates must act as if they have accepted certain moral obligations. They must not voluntarily betray the se crets of the team, whether from self-interest or principle. They must not exploit their presence in the front region in order to stage their own show, a s do, for example, marriageable stenographers who som etim es encumber their office surroundings with a lush undergrowth of high fashion. Nor must they u se their performance time as an occasion to denounce their team. They must be willing to accept minor parts with good grace and perform enthusiastically whenever, wherever, and for whomsoever the team a s a whole chooses. And they must be taken in by their own performance to the degree that is necessary to prevent them from sounding hollow and false to the audience. Perhaps the key problem in maintaining loyalty of team members (and apparently with members of other types of 135 co llectiv ities, too) is to prevent the performers from becoming so sym pathetically attached to the audience that the performers d isc lo se co them the consequences for chem of the im pression they are being given, or in other ways make the team a s a whole pay for th is attachm ent. In small communities in Britain, for example, the m anagers of sto res will often be loyal to the establishm ent and will define the product being sold to a custom er in glowing term s linked by false advice, but clerk s can frequently be found who not only appear to take the role of the customer in giving buying-advice but actually do so. 1 So, too, filling station m anagers som etim es disapprove of tipping because it may lead attendants to give undue free service to the chosen few while other custom ers are left waiting. O ne b asic technique the team can employ to defend its e lf ag a in st such disloyalty is to develop high ingroup solidarity within the team, while creating a backstage image of the audience which m akes the audience sufficiently inhuman to allow the performers to cozen them with emotional and moral immunity. To the degree that team-mates and their colleagues form a com plete so cial community which offers each performer a place and a source of moral support regardless of whether or not he is su c ce ssfu l in m aintaining his front before the audience, to that degree it would seem that performers can protect them selves from doubt and guilt and practise any kind of deception. P erh ap s we are to understand the h eartless artistry of the Thugs by reference to the religious beliefs and ritual practices into which their depredations were integrated, and perhaps we are to understand the successful ca llo u sn e ss of con men by reference to their so cial solidarity in what they call the 'i l l e g i t ’ world and their well-formulated denigrations of the legitim ate world. P erhaps chis notion allow s u s to understand in part why groups that are alien ated from or not yet incorporated into the community are so able to go into dirty-work trades and into the kind of service occupations which involve routine cheating. A second technique for counteracting the danger of affective tie s between performers and audience is to change audiences periodically. T hus filling statio n managers used to be shifted periodically from one sta tio n to another to prevent the formation of strong personal tie s with particular clients. It was found that when such tie s were allowed to form, th e manager l In the islan d community referred to in this report, I heard a clerk say to a customer a s the clerk was handing over a bottle of cherry pop to him, <( I do not se e how you can drink that stuff.” No one p resen t considered this to be surprising frankness, and sim ilar comments could be heard every day in the shops on the island. 136 sometimes placed the in te rests of a friend who needed credit before the in terests of the social establishm ent. 1 Bank m anagers and m inisters have been routinely shifted for sim ilar reasons, a s have certain colonial adm inistrators. Some female professionals provide another illustration, as the following reference to organized prostitution su g g e s ts : T h e Syndicate handles th at these days. The girls don’t sta y in one placc long enough to really pet on speaking term s with anybody- T h ere^ not so much chance of a girl falling in love with som e guy—you know, and cau sin g a squawk. Anyway, the hustler who's in Chicago this week is in St. Louis nextt or moving around to half a dozen p la c e s in town before being sent somewhere else. And they never know where they're going until they're told. 2 2. DRAMATURGICAL DISCIPLIN E. It is crucial for the m aintenance of the team ’s performance that each member of the team p o s s e s s dramaturgical discipline,, and exercise it in presenting his own part. I refer to the fact that while the performer is ostensibly immersed and given over to the activity he is performing, and is apparently engrossed in h is actions in a spontaneous, uncalculating way, he must none the le s s be affectively dissociated from his presentation in a way that leav es him free to cope with dramaturgical contingencies as they arise. He must offer a show of intellectual and emotional involvement in the activity he is piesenting, but must keep himself from actually being carried away by his own show le st this destroy his involvement in the task of putting on a successful performance. A performer who is disciplined, dramaturgically speaking, is someone who remembers his part and-does not commit unmeant gestures or faux p as in performing it. He is someone with d iscretion: he does not give the show away by involuntarily disclosing its secrets. He is someone with 'p re se n c e of m ind’ who can cover up on the spur of the moment for inappropriate behaviour on the part of his team-mates, while all the time maintaining the im pression that he is merely playing his part. And if a disruption of the performance cannot be avoided or concealed, the disciplined performer will be prepared to offer a plausible reason for discounting the disruptive event, a joking manner to remove its importance, or deep apology and self-abasem ent to reinstate those held responsible for it. The disciplined performer is algo someone with 'self-co n tro l.' 'O f course this betrayal is system atically faked in some commercial establish m en ts where the customer is given a ' special * cut price by a clerk who claims to be doing this in order to secure the buyer as a steady personal customer. 2 Charles Hamilton, Men o f the Underworld (New York: Macmillan, 1952), p. 222. 137 He can suppress his emotional response to his private problems, to h is team-mates when they make m istakes, and to the audience when they induce untoward affection or hostility in him. And he can stop him self from laughing about matters which are defined a s serious and stop him self from taking seriously m atters defined a s humorous. In other words, he can suppress his spontaneous feelings in order to give the appearance of sticking to the affective line, the expressive status quo, e s ta b lish e d by his team ’s performance, for a display of proscribed affect may not only lead to improper disclo su res and offence to the working c o n sen su s but may also implicitly extend to the audience the sta tu s of team member. And the disciplined performer is someone with sufficient poise to move from private p la c e s of informality to public ones of varying degrees of formality, without allowing such changes to confuse him. 1 P e rh a p s the focus of dramaturgical discipline is to be found in the management of one’s face and voice. Here is the crucial test of one’s ability as a performer. Actual affective resp o n se must be concealed and an appropriate affective resp o n se must be displayed. T easing, it often seem s, is an informal initiation device employed by a team to train and te s t the capacity of its new members to 'ta k e a joke,’ that is , tn su stain a friendly manner while perhaps not feeling it. When an individual p a s s e s such a te s t of expression-control, whether he receiv es it from his new team-mates in a spirit of jest or from an unexpected n ec essity of playing in a serious performance, he can thereafter venture forth as a player who can trust himself and be trusted by others. A very nice illustration of this is given in a forthcoming paper by Howard S. B ecker on marijuana smoking. Becker reports that the irregular user of the drug has a great fear of finding himself, while under the influence of the drug, in the immediate presence of parents or work a s so c ia te s who will expect an intimate undrugged performance from him. Apparently the irregular user d o es not become a confirmed regular user until he le a m s he can be ' high ’ and yet carry off a performance before nonsm okers without betraying him self. The same iss u e arises, perhaps in a le s s dramatic form, in ordinary family life, when a decisio n has to be reached a s to the point in their training at which young members of the team can be taken to public and semi-public cerem onies, for only when the child is ready to keep control of his temper will he be a trustworthy participant on such o ccasions. 1 For an example sec P ag e, op. cil., pp. 91-92. 138 3. DRAMATURGICAL CIRCUMSPECTION- Loyalty and discipline, in the dramaturgical se n se of th ese terms, are attributes required of team-mates if the show they put on is to be sustained. In addition, it will be useful if the members of the team exercise foresight and design in determining in advance how best to stag e a show. Prudence m ust be exercised. When there is little chance of being seen, opportunities for relaxation can be tak en ; when there is little chance of being put to a test, the cold facts can be presented in a glowing light and the performers can play their part for all it is worth, investing ic with full dignity. If no care and honesty are exercised, then disruptions are likely to occur; if rigid care and honesty are exercised, then the performers are not likely to be understood ' only too well ’ but they may be misunderstood, insufficiently understood, or greatly limited in what they can build out of the dramaturgical opportunities open to them. In other words, in the interests of the team, performers will be required to ex ercise prudence and circum spection in staging the show, preparing m advance for likely contingencies and exploiting the opportunities that remain. The ex ercise or expression of dramaturgical circum spection takes well-known forms; some of these techniques for managing im pressions will be considered hert. Obviously, one such technique is for the team to choose members who are loyal and disciplined, and a second one is lor the team to acquire a clea r idea as to how much loyalty and discipline it can rely on from the membership a s a whole, for the degree to which these attributes are p o sse sse d will markedly affect the likelihood of carrying off a performance and hence the safety of investing the performance with serio u sn ess, weight, and dignity. We will also find that the circumspect performer will attempt to se le c t the kind of audience that will give a minimum of trouble in terms of the show the performer wants to put on and the show he does not want to have to put on. Thus it is reported that teachers often favour neither low er-class pupils nor upper-class ones, because both groups may make it difficult to maintain in the classroom the kind of definition of the situation which affirms the professional teacher role. 1 T eachers will transfer to m iddle-class schools for th e se dramaturgical reasons. So, too, it is reported that some nurses like to work in an operating room rather than on a ward because in the operating room m easures are taken to ensure that the audience, who numbers only one, is soon oblivious to the 1 Becker, 'S o c ia l C la s s V ariations . . op. cit., pp. 461-462. 139 w e a k n esse s of the show, permitting the operating team to relax and devote its e lf to the technological requirem ents of actions a s opposed to the dramaturgical ones. 1 Once the audience is asleep it is even possible to bring in a 'g h o st su rg eo n ’ to perform the task s that others who were there will later claim to have done. 2 Similarly, given the fact that husband and wife are required to express marital solidarity by both showing the sam e regard for those whom they entertain, it is n ec essary to exclude from their g u ests those persons about whom husband and wife feel differently. 3 So also, if a man of influence and power is to make sure that he can take a friendly role in office interactions, then it will be useful for him to have a private elevator and protective circles of recep tio n ists and se creta rie s so that no one can get in to see him whom he might have to treat in a h eartless or snobbish fashion. It will be apparent that an automatic way of ensuring that no member of the team or no member of the audience a c ts improperly is to limit the siz e of both team s a s much a s possible. Other things being equal, the fewer the members, the le s s p o ssibility of m istakes, ’ d iffic u ltie s,’ and treacheries. T hus salesm en like to sell to unaccompanied custom ers, sin c e it is generally thought that two persons in the audience are much more difficult to 1sell ’ than one. So, too, in some schools there is an informal rule that no teacher is to enter the room of another teach er while the other i s holding a c l a s s ; apparently the assum ption is that it will be likely the new performer will do som ething that the waiting ey es of the student audience will se e a s in c o n sisten t with the im pression fostered by their own teacher. 4 However, there are at le ast two reasons why th is device of limiting the number of persons present h as lim itations itself. F irst, some perform ances cannot be presented without the technical a s sis ta n c e of a siz ea b le number of teammates. T hus, although a general staff ap p reciates that the more officers there are who know the plans for the next phase of action, the more likelihood that someone will act in such a 'U n p u b lish ed research report by Edith L entz. It may be noted that the policy som etim es followed of piping music by earphones to the patient who is undergoing an operation without a general an esth etic is a means of effectively removing him a s an audience for the spoken word. 2 Solomon, op. cit., p. 108. 3 T his point h as been developed in a short story by Mary McCarthy, 'A F riend of the F am ily,’ reprinted in Mary McCarthy, Cast A Cold E ye (New Y otk : Harcourt Brace, 1950). 4 Becker, 'T h e T each et in the Authority System of the Public School,’ op. cit., p. 139. 140 way as to d isc lo se strategic se crets, the staff will still have to let enough men in on the secret to plan and arrange the event. Secondly, it appears that individuals, as pieces of expressive equipment, are more effective in a certain sen se than non-human parts of the setting. If, then, an individual is to be given a place of great dramatic prominence, it may be necessary to employ a sizeab le court-following to achieve an effective impression of adulation around him. 1 have suggested that by keeping close to the facts it may be possible for a performer to safeguard his show but this may prevent him from staging a very elaborate one. If an elaborate show is to be safely staged it may be more useful to remove oneself from the facts rather than stick to them. It is feasible for an official of a religion to conduct a solemn, awesome presentation, because there is no recognized way by which th ese claim s can be discredited. Similarly, the professional takes the stand that the service he performs is not to be judged by the results it achieves but by the degree to which available occupational sk ills have been proficiently applied; and, of course, the professional claim s that only the colleague group can make a judgment of this kind. It is there* fore possible for the professional to commit himself fully to his presentation, with all his weight and dignity, knowing that only a very foolish m istake will be capable of destroying the im pression created. Thus we can understand the effort of tradesmen to obtain a professional mandate a s an effort to gain control over the reality they present to their custom ers; and in turn we can see that such control makes it unnecessary to be prudently humble in the airs one assu m es in performing one’s trade. There would appear to be a relation between the amount of modesty employed and the temporal length of a performance. If the audience is to see only a brief performance, then the likelihood of an em barrassing occurrence will be relatively small, and it will be relatively safe for the performer, especially in anonymous circum stances, to maintain a front that is rather false. 1 In American society there is what is called a ‘ telephone voice,' a cultivated form of speech not employed in face-to-face talk because of the danger of doing so. In Britain, in the kinds of contact between strangers that are 1 In brief anonymous service relations, servers become skilled at detecting what they se e a s affectation, and sin c e their own position is made clear by their service role they cannot return affectation with affectation. At the same time, customers who are what they claim to be often se n se that the server may not appreciate this, and so the customer may feel ashamed because he feels as he would feel were he as false a s he appears to be. 141 guaranteed to be very brief—the kinds involving 'please,* ‘ thank you,’ 'e x c u s e m e,’ .and 'm ay I speak t o ’—one hears many more P u b lic School a c c e n ts than there are P ublic School people. So also, in A nglo-A m erican society, the majority of dom estic estab lish m en ts do not p o sse ss sufficient staging equipment to maintain a show of polite hospitality for g u ests who stay more than a few hours; only in the upper-middle and upper c la s s e s do we find the institution of the week-end guest, for it is only here that performers feel they have enough sign equipment to bring off a lengthy show. 1 T he performer who is to be dramaturgically prudent will have to adapt his performance to the information conditions under which it must be staged. O bviously, he will have to tak e into consideration the information the audience already p o s s e s s e s about him. T he more information the audience h as about the performer, the le s s likely it is that anything they le am during the interaction will radically influence them. On th e other hand, where no prior information is p o sse sse d , it may be expected that the information gleaned during the interaction will be of relatively great importance. Hence, on the whole, we may expect individuals to relax the strict maintenance of front when they are with those they have known for a long time, and we may expect performers to tighten their front when among p erso n s who are new to them. With those whom one does not know, careful performances are required. Another condition asso cia ted with communication may be cited. The circum spect performer will have to consider th t audience’s a c c e s s to information sources external to the interaction. F o r example, members of the Thug tribe of India are said to have given the following perform ances during the early nineteenth century: As a general rule chcy ptetended to be m erchants or soldiers, travelling without weapons in order co disarm suspicion, which gave them an ex cellent excuse for seeking perm ission co accompany travellers, for chere was nothing to ex cite alarm in thetr appearance. Mosc Thugs were mild looking and peculiarly courteous, for th is camouflage formed part of cheir stock-in-ttade, and well-armed travellers felt no fear in allowing th ese knights of che road co join them. T h is first step su c cessfu lly accom plished, che Thugs gradually won the confidence of cheir intended vtccims by a demeanour of humilicy and gratitude, and feigned incercst in their affairs until familiar wich details of their homes, whether they were likely to be m issed if murdered, and if they knew anyone in the vicinity. Sometimes they travelled long distan c es together before a su itable opportunicy foe treachery occurred; a ca s e is on record where a gang journeyed with a family of eleven persons for 1 On the island studied by the wricer, some crofcers felc they could suscain a m iddle-class show for the duration of a tea, in some cases a meal, and in* one or two c a s e s even a week-end; but many islanders felt it only sa fe to perform for m iddle-class au dien ces on che front porch or, betcer still, in che community hall, where (he efforts and resp o n sib ilities of the show could be shared by many team-mates. 142 twenty days, covering 200 m iles, before they succeeded in murderins the whole parry without detection. 1 0 Thugs could give th ese performances in spice of the fact that their audiences were constantly on the watch for such performers (and quickly put to death those identified as Thugs) partly because of the informational conditions of travel; once a party set out for a distant destination, there was no way for them to check the iden tities claimed by those whom they encountered, and if anything befell the party on the way it would be months before they would be considered overdue, by which time the Thugs who had performed for and then upon them would be out of reach. But in their native villages, the members of the tribe, being known, fixed, and accountable for their sins, behaved in an exemplary fashion. Similarly, circum spect Americans who would ordinarily never chance a m isrepresentation of their social sta tu s may take such a chance while staying for a short time a t a summer resort. If so u rces of information external to the interaction constitute one contingency the circum spect performer must take into consideration, sources of information internal to the interaction constitute another. Thus the circum spect performer will adjust h is presentation according to the character of the props and ta s k s out of which he must build his performance. For example, clothing m erchants in the United S tates are required to be relatively circumspect in making exaggerated claim s, because custom ers can test by sight and touch whar is shown for them, but furniture s a le s men need not be so careful, because few members of the* audience can judge what lies behind the front of varnish and veneer that is presented to them. 2 Similarly, if a housewife is concerned with showing that she maintains clean lin ess standards, she is likely to focus her attention upon the g la s s surfaces in her living room, for g la s s shows dirt all too clearly; she will give le s s attention ro the darker and le s s revealing rug, which may well have been chosen in the belief that 'd ark colours do not show the dirt.’ So, too, an artist need take little care with the decor of his studio—in fact, the a rtist’s studio has become stereotyped as a place where those who work backstage do not care who s e e s them or the conditions in which they are s e e n —partly because the full value of the artist’s product can, or ought to be, immediately available to the s e n s e s ; portrait painters, on the other hand, must promise to make the sittings satisfactory and tend to use relatively l Sleeman, op. cil., p. 25. 2 Conant, op. cit., makes this point. 143 prep o ssessin g , rich-looking stu d io s as a kind of guarantee for the prom ises they make. Similarly, we find that confidence men must employ elaborate and meticulous personal fronts and often engineer m eticulous so cial settings, not so much because they lie for a living but because, in order to get away with a lie of that dimension, one must deal with persons who have been and are going to be scrangers, and one h a s to term inate the dealings as quickly a s possible, Legitim ate businessm en who would promote .n venture under these circum stances would have to be just a s m eticulous in expressing them selves, for it is under just such circum stances that potential investors scrutinize the character of those who would sell to them. In short, sin ce a con merchant must sw indle his c lie n ts under th o se circum stances where c lien ts appreciate that a confidence gam e could be employed, the con man must carefully forestall the immediate impression that he might be what in fact he is, just a s the legitim ate merchant, under the sam e circum stances, would have to forestall carefully the immediate impression that he might be what he is not. It i s apparent that care will be great in situations where important consequences for the performer will occur as a result of his conduct. T he job-interview is a clear example. Often the interviewer will have to make d ec isio n s of far-reaching imporrance for the interview ee on the sole b a s is of information gained from the interview ee’s interview-performance. The interview ee is likely to feel, and with some justice, that his every action will be taken a s highly symbolical, and he will therefore give much preparation and thought to his performance. We expect at such tim es that the interviewee will pay much attention to his appearance and manner, not merely to create a favourable im pression, but also to be on the safe side and forestall any unfavourable im pression that might be unwittingly conveyed. Another example may be suggested: those who work in the field of radio broadcasting and, especially, television keenly appreciate that the momentary impression they give will have a la stin g effect on the view the audience tak es of them, and it is in this part of the communication industry that great care is taken to give the right impression and great anxiety is felt that the im pression given might not be right. The strength of this concern is seen in the indignities that high-placed performers are willing to suffer in order to come off well : Congressm en allow them selves to be made up and to be told what to w ear; professional boxers ab a se them selves by giving a display, in the manner of w restlers, instead of a bout. 1 1 See John Lardner's weekly column in N ew sw eek, February 22, 1954, p. 59. *44 Circum spection on the part of performers will also be expressed in the way they handle relaxation of appearances. When a team is physically distant from its inspectorial audience and a surprise visit is unlikely, then great relaxation becomes feasible. T hus we read that small American Navy in stallatio n s on P acific isla n d s during the la st war could be run quite informally, whereas a readjustment in the direction of spit and polish was required when the outfit moved to p l a t e s that members of the audience were more likely to frequent. 1 When in sp ecto rs have easy a c c e s s to the place where a team carries on its work, then the amount of relaxation possible for the team will depend on the efficiency and reliability of its warning system. It is to be noted that thorough-going relaxation requires not only a warning system but also an appreciable time lap se between warning and visit, for the team will be able to relax only to the degree that can be corrected during such a time lapse. Thus, when a schoolteacher leav es her classroom for a moment, her charges can relax into slovenly postures and whispered conversations, for th e se tran sg ressio n s can be corrected in the few seco n d s' warning the pupils will have that the teacher is about to re-enter, but it is unlikely that it will be feasible for the pupils to sneak a smoke, for the smell of smoke cannot be got rid of quickly. Interestingly enough, pupils, like other performers, will ‘te s t the lim its,’ gleefully moving far enough away from their s e a ts so that when the warning com es they will have to dash madly back to their proper p laces so as not to be caught off-base. I would like to mention a final way in which, dramaturgical circumspection is exercised. When team s come into each other's immediate presence, a host of minor events may occur that are accidentally suitable for conveying a general impression that is in co n sisten t with the fostered one. T his expressive treacherousness is a basic ch aracteristic of face-to-face interaction. One way of dealing with this problem is, ^as previously suggested, to select team -m ates who are disciplined and will not perform their parts in a clumsy, gauche, or self-conscious fashion. Another method i s to prepare in advance for all possible expressive contingencies. One application of this strategy is to settle on a complete agenda before the event, designating who is to do what and who is to do what after that. In this way confusions and lulls can be avoided and hence the im pressions that such hitches in the proceedings might convey to the audience can be avoided too. Another application of this programming technique is to accept the fact that picayune ev en ts such a s who is to enter a room first 1Page, op. cit., p. 92. 145 or who is to sit next to th e h o ste ss, etc., will be taken a s ex p ressio n s of regard and to apportion these favours consciously on the b a s is of p rinciples of judgment to which no one present will tak e offence, such as age, gross seniority in rank, sex, temporary ceremonial statu s, etc. T h u s in an important sense protocol i s not so much a device for expressing valuations during interaction a s a device for 'grounding* potentially disruptive ex p ressio n s in a way that will be acceptable (and uneventful) to all present. A third application i s to rehearse the whole routine so that the performers can become practised in their parts and so that contingencies that were not predicted will occur under circum stances in which they can be safely attended to. A fourth i s to outline beforehand for the audience the line of response they are to take to the performance. When this kind of briefing occurs, of course, it becom es difficult to distinguish between performers and audience. T his type of co llu sio n i s esp ecially found where the performer is of highly sacred sta tu s and cannot trust himself to the spontaneous ta c t of the audience. F o r example, in B ritain, women who are to be presented at court (whom we may think of a s an audience for the royal performers) are carefully schooled beforehand a s to what to wear, what kind of lim ousine to arrive in, how to curtsey, and what to say. Protective Practices I have suggested three attrib u tes that team members must have if their team is to perform in safety ; loyalty, discipline, and circum spection. E ach of th e se c a p a c itie s is expressed in many standard defensive techniques through which a set of performers can sa v e their own show. Some of these techniques of im pression management were reviewed. O thers, such a s the practice of controlling a c c e s s to back regions and front regions, were sufficiently d isc u sse d in earlier chapters. In th is section I want to s tr e s s the fact that most of th ese defensive techniques of im pression management have a counterpart in the tactful tendency of the audience and outsiders to act in a protective way in order to help the performers save their own show. Since the dependence of the performers on the tact of the audience and o u tsid ers tends to be underestim ated, I shall bring together here some of the several protective techniques th a t are commonly employed although, analytically speaking, each protective practice might better be considered in conjunction with the corresponding defensive p ractice. 146 F irst, it should be understood that a c c e s s to the back and front regions of a performance i s controlled not only by the performers but by others. Individuals voluntarily stay away from regions into which they have not been invited. (T his kind of tact in regard to p lace is analagous to 'd is c re tio n ,’ which h as already been described a s tact in regard to facts.) And when outsiders find they are about to enter such a region, they often give th o se already present some warning, in the form of a m essage, or a knock, or a cough, so that the intrusion can be put off if necessary or the setting hurriedly put in order and proper expressions fixed on the fac es of those present. 1 T h is kind of tact can become nicely elaborated. Thus, in presenting oneself to a stranger by m eans of a letter of introduction, it is thought proper to convey the letter to the addressee before actually coming into his immediate p resen ce; the addressee then h as time to decide what kind of greeting the individual is to receive, and time to assem ble the expressive manner appropriate to such a greeting. 2 We often find that when interaction must proceed in the presence of outsiders, outsiders tactfully act in an uninterested, uninvolved, unperceiving fashion, so that if physical closure i s not obtained by w alls or distance, effective closure can at le ast be obtained by convention. T hus when two s e ts of persons find them selves in neighbouring booths in a restaurant, it is expected that neither group will avail itself of the opportunities that actually exist for overhearing the other. E tiquette as regards tactful inattention, and the effective privacy it provides, varies, of course, from one society and subculture to another. In m iddle-class Anglo-American society, when in a public place, one is supposed to keep one’s nose out of other people’s activity and go about o n e’s own business. It is only when a woman drops a package, or when a fellowmotorist gets stalled in the middle of the road, or when a baby left alone in a carriage begins to scream, that m iddle-class people feel it is all right to break down momentarily the walls which effectively insulate them. In the rural island culture studied by the writer, different rules obtained. If any man happened to find himself in the presence of others who were engaged in a task , it was expected that he would lend a hand, 1 Maids are often trained to enter a room without knocking, or to knock and go right in, presumably on the theory that they arc non-persons before whom any pretence or interaction readin ess on the part of those in the room need not be maintained. Friendly housewives will enter each other’s k itchens with sim ilar licence, as an expression of having nothing to hide from each other. * Esquire E tiquette, op. cit., p. 73- 147 es p ec ially if the task was relatively brief and relatively strenuous. Such casu al mutual aid was taken as a matter of course and was an expression o f nothing closer than fellowislan d er status. Once the audience has been admitted to a performance, the n e c e s sity of being tactful does not cease. We find that there is an elaborate etiquette by which individuals guide them selves in their capacity as members of the audience. T h is in v o lv e s: the giving of a proper amount of attention and in te rest; a w illingness to hold in check one's own performance so as not to introduce too many contradictions, interruptions, or demands for attention ; the inhibition of all a c ts or statem ents that might create a faux p a s ; the desire, above *li else, to avoid a scene. Audience tact is so general a thing that we may expect to find it exercised even by individuals, famous for their misbehaviour, who are patients in mental hospicals. T h u s one research group reports: Ac another time, the staff, without consulting the patients, decided to give them a V alentine party. Many of the patients did not wish to go, but did so anyway a s they felc that they should not hurt the feelings of che student n u rses who had organized the party. The gam es introduced by the n urses were on a very childish level; many of che paciencs felt silly playing them and were glad when the party was over and they could go back to activ ities of their own choosing. 1 We also find that when performers make a slip of some kind, clearly exhibiting a discrepancy between the fostered im pression and a disc lo sed reality, the audience may tactfully 'n o t s e e ’ the slip or readily accept the excuse that is offered for it. Further, we find that a t moments of crisis for the performers, the whole audience may come into tacit collusion with the performers in order to help them out. T h u s we learn th a t in mental h o spitals when a patient d ies in such a way a s to reflect upon the impression of useful treatment that the staff is attem pting to maintain, the other patients, ordinaiily disposed to give the sta ff trouble, will tactfully e a s e up their warfare and with much delicacy help sustain the quite fal^e im pression that they have not absorbed the meaning of what h a s happened. 2 Similarly, at tim es of inspection, whether in school, in barracks, in the hospital, or at home, the audience is likely to behave itself in a model way so that the performers who are being inspected may put on an exemplary show. At 'W illiam Caudill, Frederick C. Redlicli, Helen R. Gilmore and Kugene B. Brody, ‘S ocial Strucrure and Interaction P ro c e ss e s on a Psychiatric Ward,’ American Journal o f Orthopsychiatry, XXII, 321-322. 2 S ee T ax el, op. cit., p. 118. Vl'hen two teams know an em barrassing fact, and each team knows the other team knows it, and yet ncM ier team openly admits its knowledge, we get an in stan c e of what Robert bubin has called 'o rg anization al fiction s.' See Dubin, op. cit., pp. 341-3^5- 148 such tim es, team lin e s are apt to shift slightly and momentarily so that the inspecting superintendent, general, director, or guest will be faced by performers and audience who are in collusion. A linal instance of tact in handling the performer may be cited. When the performer is known to be a beginner, and more su b je ct than otherw ise to em barrassing m istakes, the audience, frequently show s extra consideration, refraining from causing the difficulties it might otherw ise create. I would like to add a concluding fact about tact. Whenever the audience ex e rcises tact, the possibility will arise that the performers will learn that they are being tactfully protected. When this occurs, the further possibility arise s that the audience will learn that the performers know they are being tactfully protected. And then, in turn, it becomes possible for the performers to learn that the audience knows that the performers know they are being protected. Now when such s ta te s of information exist, a moment in the performance may come when the se p arate n ess of the team s will break down and be momentarily replaced by a communion of glances through which ea ch team openly adm its to the other its state of information. At such moments the whole dramaturgical structure of social interaction is suddenly and poignantly laid bare, and the line separating the team s momentarily disappears. Whether this clo se view of things brings shame or laughter, the teams» are likely to draw rapidly back into their appointed characters. Tact Regarding Tact It h a s been argued that the audience contributes in a significant way to the m aintenance of a show by exercising tact or protective p ractices on behalf of the performers. It is apparent that if the audience is to employ tact on the performer’s behalf, the performer must act in such a way a s to make the rendering of th is a s s is ta n c e possible. T h is will require discip lin e and circum spection, but of a sp e cia l order. For example, it w as suggested that tactful outsid ers in a physical position to overhear an interaction may offer a show of inattention. In order to a s s is t in this tactful withdrawal, the p articip an ts who feel it is physically p o ssible for them to be overheard may omit from their conversation and activity anything that would tax this tactful resolve of the outsiders, and at the sam e time include enough sem i-confidential facts to show th at they do not d istrust the show of withdrawal presented by the outsiders. Similarly, it a secretary is to tell a visitor 149 tactfully that the man he w ishes to see is out, it will be wise for the visitor to step back from the inter-office telephone so that he cannot hear what the secretary is being told by the man who is presumably not there to tell her. I would like to conclude by mentioning two general strateg ies regarding tact with respect to tact. F irst, the performer must be sensitive to hints and ready to take them, for it is through hints that the audience can warn the performer that his show is unacceptable and that he had better modify it quickly if the situation is to be saved. Secondly, if the performer is to m isrepresent the facts in any way, he must do so in accordance with the etiquette for m isrepresentation; he must not leav e himself in a position from which even the lam est excuse and the most co-operative audience cannot extricate him. In telling an untruth, the performer is enjoined to retain a shadow of jest in his voice so that, should he be caughc out, he can disavow any claim to serio u sn ess and say that he was only joking. In m isrepresenting his physical appearance, the performer is enjoined to use a method which allows of an innocent excuse. Thus balding men who affect a hat indoors and out are more or le s s excused, since it is po ssib le that they have a cold, chat they, merely forgot to take their hat off, or that rain can fall in unexpected p la c e s ; a toupee, however, offers the wearer no excuse and the audience no excuse for excuse. In fact there is a sense in which the category of impostor, previously referred co, can be defined as a person who makes it im possible for his audience to be tactful about observed m isrepresentation. aranniD In spite of the fact that performers and audience employ all of these techniques of impression management, and many others as well, we know, of course, that incidents do occur and that audiences are inadvertantly given glim pses behind the sc en e s of a performance. When such an incident occurs, the members of an audience sometimes learn an important lesson, more important to them than the aggressive pleasure they can obtain by discovering someone’s dark. • PQf'i^red. inside, or scrategic secrets. The members of the audience may discover a fundamental democracy that is usually well hidden. Whether the character that is being presented is sober or carefree, of high station or low, the individual who performs the character will be seen for what he largely is, a solitary player involved in a harried concern for his production. Behind 150 many m asks and many characters, each performer tends to wear a single look, a naked unsocialiEed look, a look of concentration, a look of one who is privately engaged in a difficult, treacherous task. De Beauvoir, in her book on women, provides an illu stratio n : And in spice o f all her prudence, accidents will happen; wine i s s D i l i e d on her d ress, a cigarette burns i t ; th is marks the disappearance of the luxurious and festive creature who bore herself with smiling oride in the ballroom, fot she now assu m es the serious and severe look of the ho usekeeper; it becom es all at once evident that her toilette was not a se t piece like fireworks, a transient burst of splendor, intended for the lavish illumination of a moment. It is rather a rich possessio n , capital goods, an investm ent; it has meant sa crifice; its lo ss i s a real d isa ste r Spots, rents, botched dressm aking, bad h aird o s are catastrophes still more serious than a burnt roast or a broken v a s e ; for not onlv do es the woman of fashion project h erself into things, she h as chosen to make herself a thing, and she feels directly threatened in rhe world Her relations with dressm aker and milliner, her fidgeting, her strict d e m a n d s-a ll these m anifest her serious att.tude and her sense of insecurity. 1 _______________ __________________ i de Beauvoir, op. cil., p. 536. 151 CHAPTER Vil CONCLUSION The Framework A social establishm ent is any place surrounded by fixed barriers to perception in which a particular kind of activity regularly takes place. I have suggested that any so cial establishm ent may be studied profitably from the point of view of impression management. Within the w alls of a social establishm ent we find a team of performers who co-operate to p resent to an audience a given definition of the situation. T h is will include the conception of own team and of audience and assum ptions concerning the ethos that is to be m aintained by rules of p o liten e ss and decorum. We often find a division into back region, where the performance of a routine is prepared, and front region, where the performance is presented. We find that a c c e s s to these regions is controlled in order to prevent the audience from seeing backstage and to prevent outsiders from coming into a performance that is not addressed to them. Among members of the team we find that familiarity prevails, that solidarity is likely to develop, and that se c rets that could give the show away are shared and kept. A tacit agreement i s maintained between performers and audience to act as if a given degree of opposition and of accord existed between them. T ypically, but not alw ays, agreement is stressed and opposition is underplayed. We find that the resulting working co n sen su s tends to be contradicted by the attitude toward the audience which the performers express in the absence of the audience aijd by carefully controlled communication out of character conveyed by the performers while the audience is present. We find that discrepant roles develop: some of the individuals who are apparently team-mates, or audience, or outsiders acquire information about the performance and relations to the team which are not apparent and which com plicate the problem of putting on a show. We find that sometim es disruptions occur through unmeant gestures, faux pas, and scen e s, thus discrediting or contradicting the definition of the situation that is being maintained. We find that the mythology of the team will dwell upon th ese disruptive events. We find that performers, audience, and outsiders all 152 utilize techniques for saving the show, whether by avoiding likely disruptions or by correcting for unavoided ones, or by making it possible for others to do so. Tc ensure that these techniques will be employed, we find that the team will tend to se lec t members who are loyal, disciplined, and circum spect, and to se lec t an audience that is tactful. T hese features and elements, then, comprise the framework 1 claim to be characteristic of much social interaction as it occurs in natural settings in our society. This framework is formal and abstract in the se n se that it can be applied to any social establishm ent; it is not, however, merely a sta tic classification. The framework bears upon dynamic iss u e s created by the motivation to su stain a definition of the situation which has been projected before others. The Analytical Context T his report h a s been chiefly concerned with social establishm ents as relatively closed system s. It has been assumed that the relation of one establishm ent to others is itself an intelligible area of study and ought to be treated analytically as part of a different order of fact—the order of institutional integration. It might be well here to try to place the perspective taken in this report in the context of other perspectives which seem to be the ones currently employed, implicitly or explicitly, in the study of social establishm ents a s closed system s. Four such perspectives may be tentatively suggested. An establishm ent may be viewed ' tech n ically ,’ in terms of its efficiency and inefficiency as an intentionally organized system of activity for the achievement of pre-defined objectives. An establishm ent may be viewed ‘ politically,’ in terms of the actions which each participant (or c la s s of participants) can demand of other participants, the kinds of deprivations and indulgences which can be meted out in order to enforce these demands, and the kinds of so cial controls which guide this exercise of command and use of sanctions. An establishm ent may be viewed 'stru c tu ra lly ,’ in terms of the horizontal and vertical sta tu s divisions and the kinds of social relations which relate these several groupings to one another. Finally, an establishm ent may be viewed 'c u ltu ra lly ,’ in term s of the moral values which influence activity in the establishm ent—values pertaining to fashions, custom s, and matters of taste, to politeness and decorum, to ultimate ends and normative restrictions on means, etc. It is 153 t' Se noted that all the fac ts that can be discovered about an e^ a b lish m en t are relevant to each of the four perspectives but that each perspective gives its own priority and order to th ese facts. It seem s to me tbat the dramaturgical approach may constitute a fifth perspective, to be added to the technical, political, structural, and cultural perspectives. 1 The dramaturgical perspective, like each of the other four, can be employed a s the end-point of an alysis, a s a final way of ordering facts. T his would lead us to describe the techniques of im pression management employed in a given establishm ent, the principal problem s of im pression management in the establishm ent, and the identity and interrelationships of the several performance team s which operate in the establishm ent. But, as with the facts utilized in each of the other p erspectives, the facts sp ecifically pertaining to impression management also play a part in the m atters that are a concern in all the other p erspectives. It may be useful to illustrate this briefly. The technical and dramaturgical perspectives intersect most clearly, perhaps, in regard to standards of work. Important for both persp ectiv es is the fact that one se t of individuals will be concerned with te stin g the unapparent characteristics and qualities of the work-accom plishments of another set of individuals, and this other s e t will be concerned with giving the im pression that their work em bodies these hidden attributes. T he political and dramaturgical perspectives intersect clearly in regard to the ca p acities of one individual to direct the activity of another. For one thing, if an individual is to direct another, or others, he will often find it useful to keep strategic se crets from them. Further, if one individual attem pts to direct the activity of others by means of example, enlightenment, persuasion, exchange, manipulation, authority, threat, punishment, or coercion, it will be necessary , regardless of his power position, to convey effectively what he wants done and what he is prepared to do to get it done arid what he will do if it is not done. Power of any kind must be clothed in effective means of displaying it, and it will have different effects depending upon how it is dramatized. (Of course, the capacity to convey effectively a definition of the situation may be of little use if one is not in a position to give example, exchange, punishment, etc.) T hus the most objective form of naked power, i.e., physical coercion, is often neither objective nor C o m p a r e the position taken by Oswald Hall in regard to po ssib le p ersp ectiv es for the study of closed sy stem s in his ' Methods and T echniques of Research in Human R e la tio n s' (April, 1952), reported in E. C- Hughes et al., C a ses on F ield Work, forthcoming. 154 naked but rather functions as a display for persuading the audience; it is often a means of communication, not merely a means of action. The structural and dramaturgical perspectives seem to intersect most clearly in regard to social distance. The image that one statu s grouping is able to maintain in the eyes of an audience of other sta tu s groupings will depend upon the performers’ capacity to restrict communicative contact with the audience. The cultural and dramaturgical perspectives intersect most clearly in regard to the maintenance of moral standards. The cultural values of an establishm ent will determine in detail how the participants are to feel about many matters and at the same time es tab lish a framework of appearances that must be maintained, whether or not there is feeling behind the appearances. Personolity-lnteroction-Sociefy In recent years there have been elaborate attem pts to bring into one framework the concepts and findings derived from three different areas of inquiry: the individual personality, social interaction, and society. I would like to suggest here a simple addition to these inter-disciplinary attem pts. When an individual appears before others, he wittingly and unwittingly projects a definition of the situation, of which a conception of him self is an important part. When an event occurs which is expressively incompatible with this fostered impression, significant consequences are sim ultaneously felt in three levels of social reality, each of which involves a different point of reference and a different order of fact. First, the social interaction, treated here as a dialogue between two teams, may come to an em barrassed and confused halt; the situation may ce ase to be defined, previous positions may become no longer tenable, and participants may find them selves without a charted course of action. The participants typically sense a false note in the situation and come to feel awkward, flustered, and, literally, out of countenance. In other words, the minute social system created and sustained by orderly social interaction becomes disorganized. T hese are the consequences that the disruption has from the point of view of social interaction. Secondly, in addition to these disorganizing consequences for action at the moment, performance disruptions may have consequences of a more far-reaching kind. Audiences tend to accept the self projecced by the individual performer during any current performance as a responsible representative of 155 his colleague-grouping, of his team, and of his so cial establishm ent. Audiences also accept the individual’s particular performance as evidence of his capacity to perform the routine and even a s evidence of his capacity to perform any routine. In a se n s e th ese larger social u n its—teams, e sta b lish ments, e tc .—become committed every time the individual performs h is routine; with each performance the legitim acy of these u n its will tend to be tested anew and their permanent reputation put at stake. T his kind of commitment is especially strong during some performances. Thus, when a surgeon and h is nurse both turn from the operating table and the anesthetized patient accidentally rolls off the table to his death, not only is the operation disrupted in an em barrassing way, but the reputation of the doctor, as a doctor and a s a man, and also the reputation of the hospital may be weakened. T hese are the con seq u en ces that disruptions may have from the point of view of social structure. Finally, we often find that the individual may deeply involve his ego in his identification with a particular role, e sta b lish ment, and group and in his self-conception as someone who does not disrupt social interaction or let down the social units which depend upon that interaction. When a disruption occurs, then, we may find that the self-conceptions around which he has built his personality may become discredited. T h e se are consequences that disruptions may have from the point of view of individual personality. Performance disruptions, then, have consequences at three le v els of ab stractio n : personality, interaction, and social structure. While the likelihood of disruption will vary widely from interaction to interaction, and while the social importance of likely disruptions will vary from interaction to interaction, still it seem s that there is no interaction in which the particip an ts do not take an appreciable chance of being slightly em barrassed or a slight chance of being deeply humiliated. L ife may not be much of a gamble, but interaction is. Further, in so far as individuals make efforts to avoid disruptions or to correct for ones not avoided, th ese efforts, too, will have sim ultaneous co n seq u en ces at the three lev els. Here, then, we have one simple way of articulating three le v els of abstraction and three perspectives from which so cial life has been studied. Comporisons and Study In this report, use has been made of illustrations from s o c ie ties other than our Anglo-American one. In doing this I 156 did not mean to imply that the framework presented here is culture-free or applicable in the sam e areas of social life in non-Western so cieties a s in our own. We lead an indoor social life. We sp ecialize in fixed settings, in keeping strangers out, and in giving the performer some privacy in which to prepare him self for the show. Once we begin a performance, we are inclined to finish it, and we are sen sitiv e to jarring notes which may occur during it. If we are caught out in a m isrepresentation we feel deeply humiliated. Given our general dramaturgical rules and inclinations for conducting action, we must not overlook areas of life in other so c ie tie s in which other rules are apparently followed. Reports by Western travellers are filled with in stan ces in which their dramaturgical sen se was offended or surprised, and if we are to generalize to other cultures we must consider these in stan c es a s well as more favourable ones. We must be ready to se e in China th a t while actions and decor may be wonderfully harmonious and coherent in a private tea-room, extremely elaborate meals may be served in extremely plain restaurants, and shops that look like hovels staffed with surly, familiar clerk s may contain within their rec e sse s, wrapped in old brown paper, wonderfully delicate bolts of silk. 1 And among a people said to be careful to save each other’s face, we must be prepared to read th at: 1‘ortunniely the C hinese do not believe in the privacy of a home as we do. They do not mind having the whole details of their daily experience seen by everyone that cares to look. How they live, what they cat, and even the family jars that we try 10 hush up from the public are things th.it seem to be common property, and not to belong exclusively to this particular family who are most concerned. 2 And we must be prepared to see that in so c ie tie s with settled inequalitarian sta tu s sy stem s and strong religious orientations, individuals are sometimes le s s earnest about the whole civic drama than we are, and will c ro ss social barriers with brief gestures that give more recognition to the man behind the mask than we might find permissible. Furthermore, we must be very cautious in any effort to characterize our own society as a whole with respect to dramaturgical practices. For example, in current management-labour relations, we know that a team may enter joint consultation m eetings with the opposition with the knowledge that it may be necessary to give the appearance of stalking out of the meeting in a huff. Diplomatic team s are sometimes required to stage a similar show. In other words, while team s m our society are usually obliged to suppress their rage behind a working consensus, there are tim es when team s are obliged 1Macflowan, np. c itt> pp. 178-179» 3//ni< .* t,1// Ti1 L‘ piV rr.M A .V W tH V ' .1 i ' • *i> ph-M'r t**j•vi,*lr I