Rethinking Paradigms of Research in Psychology David Hiles De Montfort University, Leicester, UK Lecture Series, Brno, April 2001 Introduction Lecture 1 - Paradigms Lost w Lecture 2 - Paradigms Returned Lecture 3 - Paradigms Discerned Introduction "...the searchfor method becomes one of the most importantproblems of the entire enterprise of understandingthe uniquely humanform ofpsychological activity." Lev Vygotsky(1978 [1930]) Mindin Society. p.65 The purpose of these three lectures can be best described in terns of three basic tasks. My primary task, which was suggestedto me when I was invited to give this series of lectures, is to review a range of new approachesto psychological research. This will inevitably be a rather disparate range, but this is deliberate, because it will also serve to demonstrate the present malaise that psychologicalresearch.hasgot itself into, and the consequent problems that psychology is now facing. In addition, it will also enable me , to demonstrate the potential richness and possible diversity of the various paradigms needed for psychological inquiry. A broad survey of this wide landscape of inquiry would be rather aimless, so I have set myself two furthertasks. My secondarytask involvesidentifying the problems that psychologicalresearch currently faces, especially the controversiesthat permeate and stifle our discipline. And, the finaltask that Ihave set myself is to propose a solutionto these problems in the form of a. general framework or model to systematise the growing field of psychological inquiry. To be somewhat perverse, I will start in my first lecture with the third of these tasks, the solution, and then I will work back towards the second and first tasks over the next two lectures! During the past few years, I have formedthe opinion that psychology, as the scientific study of human behaviour and experience,has lost its way and is Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Intro -Page 1 indesperate need of re-inventing itself. A major obstacle for any such reinventionwill concernthe problem of "research methods", or the searchfor method as Vygotsky calls it. I especially want to examine the role that is to be played by qualitative inquiry in psychological research. These three lectures are concernedwith that issue, and will focus on the claim that at the root of our problems lies the insight that it is theparadigm within which research is cmied out that needs to be examined and made more explicit. Indeed, research methods and data analysis must alwaysbe secondary to the paradigm of research. This basic idea is not new in the human and social sciences, and has been persuasively proposed before in the work of Polkinghorne (1983) and Guba and Lincoln (1994). I will not be concerned here with the experimentalor natural scienceparadigm, as such. That paradigm is too well established, and besides, it fits in all too easily with the general model that I will propose. I do not regard the natural science approach itself as controversial, except with respect to its often uncritical application.What I do intend to be concerned with is the rethinking of the excfusiveplace that it occupies, and rethinking the need for other paradigms of inquiry. This is a path fairly well trodden, however, I am convinced that it is well worth another visit. But, as with all paths, familiar and unfamiliar, they changeby the season and the people who accompany us on our wanderings. As your companion on these three short tours, my intention is to stop on occasion in order to point out the landscape, and highlight the controversies and developments as I see them. Nevertheless, I need to be selective. Luckily, there are many excellent published reviews that I will simplypoint to in passing, which you canrevisit in your own time. The strategythat I will adopt is to highlightthose issues pertinent to my three major tasks. My argument involves making broad and subtle distinctions, asking you to engage in a process of discrimination and discernment of the nature of human inquiry. In my first lecture I will outline a new perspective on psychological research in the form of a general model of disciplined inquiry. The point behind this model is that we need to make a very close examination of the stages and the processes involvedin doing research. This furtherhighlights the urgent need to rethink psychologicalresearch as both a human science as well as a natural science. Hiles/Rethinking Paradigms/Intro -Page 2 In the second lecture, I will look more closely at the controversies surrounding the comparison between quantitative and qualitative approaches to research in psychology, and I will suggest a fairly radical way of dealing with this controversy. Then, I will outline a proposal I made two or three : years ago to develop a calculus for the human sciences. Such a calculushas two main threads - a discursive, and aphenomenologicalthread. For the remainder of the second lecture I will examinethe discursive calculus in some detail, highlighting the exciting developments in discourse analysis and narrative psychology. And in the third lecture, I will examine more closely the phenomenological calculus, especially with respect to several specific developments in psychological inquiry that are beginningto gain a foothold in the discipline. My point here is that, especially in the field of transpersonalpsychology, there is the possibility of reclaimingthose areas of research that psychology has now for far too long been ignoring. Most of the ideas presented here are not claimed to be completely new,they of course build and elaborate on the work of others.If there are any new ideas with some claim to originality,then these would include: the updated model of disciplined inquiry; my synthesis of qualitative methods of inquiry with the paradigm of the human sciences, designed to coexist alongside the natural sciences; and the outline of a calculus for the human sciences that attemptsto bring some order to the profusion of "methods".These ideas have evolved over several years, and have been presented at different stages of development at several conferences. They were developed particularly in response to teaching methods of inquiry to practising therapists and counsellors, and in advising postgraduates who are engaged in research fiom a discursiveand narrative perspective. 1 My ideas have developed out of invaluable conversationswith my fiends and colleagues, which include Ivo Cermak, SimonDyson, Dave Rowley, I Kevin Baker, Nigel Hamilton, Rachel Shaw, Scott Yates and many, many others. Furthermore, I am also indebted to what I have gleaned fiom the I I writings of Amedeo Giorgi, Donald Polkinghome, Clark Moustakas, Egon Guba and Yvonna Lincoln, William Braud and Roseinary Anderson, John Heron, Ron Valle, Valerie Janesick, Barney Glaser and Anselm Strauss and Juliet Corbin, ColinRobson, Peter Reason, Jonathan Smithand Rom Hank. Hiled Rethidting Paradigms/Intro -Page 3 And lastly, I too must acknowledgethe insights I have found in the writings of Arthur Schopenhauer,William Blake, Edmund Husserl, Wilhelm Wundt, Wilhelm Dilthey, Lev Vygotsky, Abraham Maslow, Paul Watzlawick, Jerome Bruner, StuartHall and many, many, many others. These are the writers who have erected many of the signposts alongthe path that I,have been followingfor the past ten years or so, and have been the biggest inspirationfor the paradigm of inquiry in which my own research interests now fall. Hiles/RethinkingParadigms/Intro -Page 4 Lecture Series. Bnlo, April 2001 Lecture 1: Paradigms Lost David Hiles De Montfort University,Leicester,UK. "Paradigmissuesare crucial;no inquirer,we maintain, ought togo about the businessof inquirywithout being clear aboutjust whatparadigm informs and guides his or her approach." Guba& Lincoln (1994) p.116 "Nowobviously, research on anythingwillyieldfindings that mirror its proceduresfor observingor measuring. Science always inventsa conforming realityinjust that way. Whenwe 'confirm' our theory by 'observations,'we devise procedures thatwillfavour the theory'splausibility. " JeromeBruner (1990)Acts of Meaning. p.104 Introduction I know of no other issue in psychology that generates more confusion, superficiality,arrogance, pig-headedness and dishonestythan the controversy over research methods. What seems to have happened is that psychologistshave formed themselves into more or less two distinct camps. They have taken up fairly entrenched positions, and proceeded to squabble, usually with very little insight or even interest into the perspectives of their opponents.The fault, if one needs to be found, lies on both sides. Those who favour a quantitative approachto research are often single-minded, arrogant, and simply wish for those advocating other approaches "to go away". While those who favour a qualitative approach seem to offer a confusingand unsystematic approach to research, and simply want to be left to themselves. All this is quite unnecessary, if a view of research asdisciplined inquiry is adopted. Hiles/ RethinkingParadigms/Lecture I -Page 5 TABLE 1.1 The eight principles of research I hold that the following eight principles are self-evident Research is an inquiry that results in an addition to knowledge The research question and findings need to be discussed in the context of prior knowledge Research always involves making assumptions - different researchers inevitably work within differentparadigms All research involves making practical and theoretical choices, and must embody a set of ethical principlesI Procedures used in the research should be systematic and rigorous, and must be clear enough for others to replicate The research findings should be clear and convincing to others Research is rarely conclusive - research usually leads to ideas for fbrther research We undertake research because we care and want to make a difference - writing-up,publication and sharing the findings are a part of the research process ~ i l e s /~ethinking~ o r u d i ~ m /~ecture1 -Page 6 At the heart of my argumentis the claim that it is theparadigms that guide and inform our research which need to be emphasized, rather than the methods of data collection and analysis per se. As Guba & Lincoln (1994) remark, in the quote at the start of this lecture, "paradigmissues are crucial."For several decadesnow, psychology has more or less tried to insist on a single paradigm, and as with all singularities, it has been taken for granted, and has been widely used and misused with little critical I examination.Many exciting and valuable paradigms have become lost to the centre-ground of our discipline. The warning, made by Bruner (1990) in the I quote above, that research findings mirror the methods and procedures of inquiry that we employ, needs to be taken very seriously. We must be open to a variety of paradigms, and not be so troubled by competingparadigms. We must try to grasp how paradigmatic choices can be made more explicit, and we must try to understand how paradigmatic assumptions exert their II influence througl~outthe entireresearch process. I I I What is research? Our field of psychology is not going to get very far, nor will we be able to i get very far here, if we don't sort out some basic common ground that we can all agree upon. Even a question such as what is research? could cause I controversy if we are not careful. It would be very helpful if we can at least I agree that a reasonable answer to this question might be that research is an addition to knowledge. This definition is intended to stressthat new knowledge is always created within the context of prior knowledge, andI therefore will always imply addingto an existingknowledge base in some way. Obviously, such a definitionwill necessarilyprovoke discussion of a 1 number of issues, particularlythe question of exactly what is the nature of human knowledge in the first place, etc. However, my focus here is on how I I a disciplined addition to knowledge can be achieved. What is the nature of the process involved in making this addition to knowledge? What procedures of inquiry can be claimedto be effective? What ways are thereI for guaranteeing that an inquiry is rigorous, systematic and convincing? Furthemore, what would be the consequences of settling forjust one method of inquiry, at the expense of others that are available? Whatever the position you take vis-a-vis the relative merits of different approaches to psychological inquiry, whether qualitative or quantitative, or some other approach, I think it is reasonable to claim that the eight principles listed in Table 1.1are self evident and can be taken as "true." Niles/ Rethinking Paradigmr/Lecture 1-Page 7 Smith, Harr6 & Van Langenhove (1995a, p.4) "RethinkmngPsychologv" 1 I Figure 1.1 Rethinkingpsychology: new vs. old paradigms HiledRethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 8 I propose to make this claim, notjust for psychology, but possibly for all scientific disciplines. These eight principlesprovide the framework in which I intend to work. I have highlighted, in bold, some of the key concepts and terms that help to clarifythe nature of the researchprocess. My claim that these principles are self-evident, releases me fiom the need to discuss them here at length, although I am aware that the implications and ramificationsof each of these principles could be exploredextensively.My intention is to devotethe main body of my three lectures to an examination of the third principle, which states: research always involves making assumptions, dzfferent researchers inevitablywork within dfferentparadigms. The search for method Psychology seems to be in a continualprocess of re-inventing itself, and this has been especiallytrue with respect to the issue of research methods. A recurring theme in the history of psychology has been the conflict between the quantitative and the more interpretativeapproaches to research. For example, a recent expressionof this can be seen in the movement to 'rethink' psychology (Smith, Ham6 & Van Langenhove,l995a; 1995b; and Harre & Steams, 1995).Their work is concerned with what has come to be 11 nd called the 2 CognitiveRevolution", or the "discursive turn", in psychology. They claim that ". .we may be on the verge of a new psychology" (Smith et a171995a,p. 3), and that this paradigm shift for psychology may be characterizedby a growing diversity of methodological approachesand theoreticalvoices, and has re-ignited the interest in the qualitativetradition of research. I have presented their summary of the underlying tensions and concerns this throws up in Figure 1.1. While I fully support their view of psychology and the general position that they advocate, it does need to be pointed out that this debate is much older than psychology itself, and this "new"psychology is not really new at all. In fact, many of these ideaswere addressedwell over a century ago, particularly in the work of Wilhelm Wundt and Wilhelm Dilthey, who were both concerned with the founding of psychology as a separatescientific discipline. Indeed, we could trace these ideas back even furtherto GiambattistaVico and his notion of a New Science (Vico, 1725). More or less, throughout the development of the social and human sciences, there have been two dominant approachesto the study of people and their socio-culturalcontext. One approachwas the model of the natural sciences, HiledRethinkingParadigm/ Lecture I -Page 9 Wilhelm Dilthey's (1883) distinction betweenthe Natural and HumanSciences Figure 1.2 Wilhelm Diltheytscontrastbetween the "sciences" Hiled Rethinking Paradip/ Lecture 1 -Page 10 and the other is the interpretativemodel, or as Dilthey (1883) called it Geisteswissenschaften(see Figure 1.2 for a summary of Dilthey's position). The natural science model draws on a positivist philosophy of science, and emphasizes a causal deterministicperspective,with quantitative forms of analysis prefened. The interpretative model draws on a human science perspective, emphasizingthe study of meaning and rule following behaviour, and favouring qualitative analysis. Although, for more than a century, it is the natural sciencemodel of psychology that has been the dominantparadigm, the interpretative model has never disappearedtotally. Some fifty or so years ago, it was seriouslyproposed again, in the work of Abraham Maslow, Carl Rogers and Rollo May, but in that case became obscured by the developmentof the humanistic, or third force in psychology. Similar attempts to bring the interpretative approach closer to centre stage can be seen in the work of Amedeo Giorgi (1970), Donald Polkinghome (1983), and perhaps most recently in Jerome Bruner's (1990) proposal for theproper study of man (see the summary in Table 1.2).So we seem to have a perennial strugglebetween these two models, with the interpretative perspective continually being pushed to the margins, if not obscurity. J Another matter that should concern us is the transferabilityof the methods of psychological inquiry in their application outside the laboratory.For I example, on the cover of his excellentbook, Real WorldResearch, Colin Robson (1993)points out that:I " . .. thoughfew students ofpsychology .. go on to be laboratorybased experimentalists, traditional degree and other courses, and the texts they use, tend to concentrate very largely on the design and analysis of laboratoryexperiments." , i He argues that graduates,practitioners, professionals go on tojobs where they are called upon to carry out some form of inquiry outside the laboratory, i.e. in the real world. In the majority of cases, it is anI interpretative, qualitative inquiry that they find themselves using. I wish to take up the challenge of the searchfor method that Lev Vygotsky makes in the quote I used at the beginning of my introduction. It is a challenge that is one of the most important problems involved in our establishingths uniquely human discipline of psychology. I believe that what needs to be recognized is that we are in real danger of forgettingthat what defines the field of psychology is not how we study, but whatwe HiledRethinkingParadigms/Lecture I -Page 11 TABLE 1.2 Bruner's proper study of man Jerome Bruner (1990) in his bookActs of Meaning outlines his ideas for "a proper study of man": The lStCognitive revolutionhas become marginal The 2ndCognitive revolution aims " . . to discover and to describeformally the meanings that human beings create out of their encounters with the world, and , . propose hypotheses about what meaning-making processes are implicated" (p. 2) there is a need to study "mind" as involving intentional states, such as believing, desiring, intending, grasping a meaning culturehas a constitutiverole by virtue of our participation in culture, meaning is renderedpublic and shared a culturalpsychology will not be preoccupied with behaviour but with situated action scientificpsychology will fare better when it recognizesthat its truths are relative to the point of view that it takes towards the human condition Hiled Rethinking Paradignts/Lecture 1 -Page 12 study. Crucially, we must not overlook the basic assumptions that we make, and the theoretical perspectives we take, upon what we are studying, and we must certainly heed Bruner's warning that we can so unwittingly devise procedures of inquiry that will favour the theories we start out with. The perspective that I, and a growing number of other psychologists, wish to take is that human behaviour is complex.People respond not to events but to the meaning of events. Explanation of human behaviour requires not only simple causal, deterministic mechanisms, but also needs to focus on the rulegoverning structures that underlie almost everythingthat we do. Human behaviour and experience are the consequenceof multi-determined factors, historically and culturally embedded. h here must be a place in psychology for "good" qualitative research (see Cresswell's attemptto spellthis out in Table 1.3). There is clearly a place for interpretative analysis, co-operative inquiry,phenomenological and transpersonal methods. The areas of psychology that are probably the most interestingraise concernswith respect to exploratory approacl~es,participatory methods, post hoc analysis, ecological validity, and real attempts to characterize the breadth, detail and rich contextualityof human events. Giorgi'sidea of a human science The one person who has almost single-handedly spelled out the importance of a human science approachto psychology is Amedeo Giorgi (1970; 1985; 1994). Giorgi (1994) asserts that: "There is no escaping thefact that thefundamental tension in the development of scientrficpsychology is between the meaning of science and the meaning of being human" (p. 90). Moreover, " . . the existing depnition of science developed in dialogue with nature rather than with humans, and so it never had to grapple with the meaning of human as haman" (p. 95). Giorgi is committed to an approach to psychology from within a human scienceparadigm, and this approach inevitably draws on research methods fkom the interpretative, qualitative inquiry tradition. Giorgi (1994) spells out three stark contrasts in the context of the natural science approach, which I would like to suggest really act as "red herrings."A red herring is sometlung HiledRethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 1-Page 13 TABLE 1.3 Cresswell'soutline for "good" qualitative research Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1 -Page 14 that misleads, or distracts &omthe main path or goal. The three contrasts that Giorgiproposes are: (A) the laboratory, (B) the causal relationship, and (C) measurement. These arepresented in Table 1.4, together with three more red herrings I have added that Giorgi seems to overlook.However, I am surehe would accept these three other red herrings. The point I want to make here is very important.These sixbasic principles are accepted almost without question in psychologicalresearch. They are seen by many as the basic requirements of "goodresearch", but I claim that there is nothing necessary about any of them. They are red herrings. As a set of basic assumptionsfor one paradigm of psychological inquiry they are fme.But there is a desperate need for an approachto psychological inquiry that sets out to study meaningful phenomena in context, using qualitative data, that is exploratory, participatory, and clearly open to all aspects of human experience and action. I I I will discuss further the human science approachthat Giorgi and others have outlined in my second lecture. In the remainder of this lecture, I want II I to discount these red herrings, and propose a general model of research that I think will go a long way in providing a solutionto the underlying problems I that we face. Why the controversy? Clearly, the controversy between quantitative and qualitative approachesto inquiry has been with us throughout the history of psychology. In the other human and social sciences the controversy has waned, but in psychology the pattern repeated each time is that, when the plea for permitting a qualitative approach is made, it is humoured perhaps a little, and then the dominant quantitative approach quietly takes over again. There is good reason to believe that this time history need not repeat itself quite so easily. Putting aside the rather pathetic squabbles that serve only to distract from the chief issues, there are two issues that do need closer attention and lie at the root of the controversy. The first issue concernsthe confusingprofusion of qualitative approaches to research (see Table 1.5).To anyone new to qualitative inquiry, a brief examination ofjournals and research methods textbooks reveals a disturbinglywide range of "qualitativemethods." In my attempts to teach this area to postgraduate students, I have over the past several years WiledRethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1 -Page 15 TABLE 1.4 The six "red herrings" of psychological research the phenomenon of interest needs to be isolated from its B. causal analysis simplification to deterministic C. measurement adoption of crude quantitative functional blindness various topics of study n.b the firstthree distinctions above are proposed by Amedeo Giorgi (1994), to which I have added three more and called "red herrings" HiledRetltinkingParadigm/ Lecture 1 -Page 16 TABLE 1.5 Some qualitativeapproaches to research An examination of a range ofjournals and research methods textbooks offers a disturbinglywide range of "qualitative methods": Grounded theory semi-structured Phenomenological inquiry Heuristic inquiry Action research Human inquiry groups Biographicalmethods Cooperative inquiry Participative inquiry Conversation analysis Ethnomethodology NaturalisticIField study Interpersonalprocess Intuitive inquiry Interpretative analysis Transpersonal/phenomenoExceptional experience Feminist research Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 17 Inquiry 4 Qualitative Inquiry Figure 1.3 The rejectedmodel of research HiledRethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1 -Page 18 A revised model of the two broad traditions: Natural Science Hiled RethinkingPamdigms/Lecture I -Page 19 developed a general framework that simplifies and gives some order to this confusing area. And the second issue is a little more radical. My proposal here is to reject the conventionalmodel of research, that divides research into either qualitative or quantitative inquiry (see Figure 1.3).Instead, it will be replaced, in the fnst instance, with the revised model (see Figure 1.4), that proposes a divisionbetween natural science and human science paradigms of inquiry. This is of course not a new idea, but it is an idea that needs resurrecting. The point here needs to be made very clear. The widely held distinction between qualitative and quantitative methods is simply a red herring. While the debate is cast in such terms, we will not make any progress. The issue is not what type of data we collect and analyze, the issue is the choice of paradigm of inquiry that we use to inform the planning and design of our research. What is Disciplined Inquiry? Recently (Eles, 1999a),I have argued that qualitative inquiry and quantitative inquiry can be seen as two broad traditions within the context of a more general model of researchwhich I have called disciplirzed inquiry (seeFigure 1.5). Both traditions draw on a wide variety of basic inquiry paradigms, and both are concerned with research that is rigorous, systematic and convincing, althoughthere may be differences in how they achieve these criteria. I therefore define scienceas disciplined inquiry, i.e. inquiry that involves a systematicprocess of finding an answer to a question, the solution to a problem, or extendingknowledge. I have decided to adopt this as my startingpoint for re-inventing psychology. The model presented here is a sligl~tlymodified and updated version of the model I published two years ago, and is presented here for the first time. The model incorporatesthe eight principles of research I discussed earlier (Table 1.I), and is designed to explorethe iinplicationsof defining science as disciplined inquiry -that involves systematic and rigorous procedures for finding answersto research questions, and therefore making an additionto knowledge. The term, disciplined inquiry, is not new, educational researchers have adopted it over thepast twenty years or so, and recently it has been taken up in psychology (l3raud & Anderson, 1998).It is probably Guba& Lincoln's (1994) three basicquestions:= Human Science 1 1Strategy Ethicalissues . I etc. .. Placingfindingsin context: lnterpretationllrnplication 0 literature review 0 future research 0 designlmethodlanalysis Writing-up& Publication Figure 1.5 A model of disciplined inquiry the most useful term available that can be used to characterizethe general features of the research process, and I do not think that its use here is at odds with its use by others. I see my own contributionas one of trying to offer a clear and structured frameworkwithin which a rneaningfhl debate about the nature of the researchprocess can take place. The emphasis I wish to make is that all human knowledge, all scientificresearch, in following a set of procedures, must begin with a group of assumptions, a set of beliefs, i.e. aparadigm. Furthermore, I think we need to get away, once and for all, fiom the situation where a whole field of research can frequently be criticized for its methodology without any consideration of the paradigm within which it falls. My own approach to disciplinedinquiry builds upon the importantwork of Polkinghorne (1983) and Guba & Lincoln (1994). Polkinghorne arguesthat the methodologicalperspective of the natural sciences overlooks the anomalous quality of human experience, and: "Thedzjflcultyfor human science arises, notfrom a need to change from oneparadigm to another, butfrom a need to resist settling down to any single paradigm "(p.9). For Pollunghome, it is not the methods of research, but the paradigm of research that is crucialto the researchprocess, and severalparadigms will be needed in a science such as psychology. Perhaps the clearest idea of what is involved in this notion of paradigm can be found in the work of Guba & Lincoln (1994). They stressthe importance of recognizing the paradigms at work across the various natural, social and human sciences. They argue that a paradigm is " .. not open toproof in any conventionalsense " (p. 108),and they propose that: ". .aparadigm may be viewed as a set of basic beliefs [or assumptions] that deals with ultimates orflrstprinciples" (p. 107). The model of disciplinedinquiry that I am proposingtakes this notion of paradigm as its startingpoint, and sees the inquiry process as one that is open to a variety of assumptions,choices,procedures, data analysis techniques, and critical reflections.The model explicitly has a "pick-andmix" structure, and accomodates, without favouring any one approacl~over Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Lecture I -Page 22 another, positivist/experimental approacl~esalongside constructivist/ interpretativeapproaches. Also, there is no need to make any explicit distinctionbetween quantitative and qualitative research, these can be seen simply as major "traditions of inquiry"that merely emphasizethe nature of the data being collected.There is also the possibility of identifyingseveral other traditions of inquiry where research practices and necessity dictates. Various traditions simply can be organized appropriatelyunder each of the five aspects (or stages). The model offers a pragmatic distinctionbetween the five basic aspects of the research process. These five aspects are:paradigms, strategies, methods, analysis, and critical reflection (see Table 1.6). The point is that all research activity must involve, implicitly or explicitly, all of these five stages. The model also sets out to clarifythe prevailing muddle between method (data collection) and analysis (of data), that is so evident in many recently published textbooks on "researcl~methods"in psychology. Another feature of the model is the explicitinclusion of the notion of strategies of inquiry. While paradigms do promote different strategies and methods of research, these are by no means exclusiveto any particular paradigmatic approach. Strategies can be thought of as issues of research design, but the emphasis here is on the choices, values and perspectives involved, rather than slavishly followingrecipes for research design laid down by habit, or the need to conform to some canonicalprinciple. This stage of explicitly formulatingstrategies in the research process provides the important bridge between the paradigm on the one hand and the methods of data collection and analysis on the other. Strategies and design issues must not be simplytaken for granted. In my originalpaper (Hiles, 1999a),I offered the conjecture that the failure to hghlight the strategies involved in research design, was the first step to losing sight of the issues concerningthe paradigm of inquiry which are fundamentalto the research process. Referringto Figure 1.5,we will now examine each of these five stages of tlie research process in some detail. ElParadigms of inquiry Following Guba & Lincoln (1994), I propose that the paradigm of inquiry is the set of basic assumptionsthat every scientist must make, which in principle are not open to proof in any sensibleway. It follows that there is no HiledRethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 23 TABLE 1.6 Five aspects of the research process The model of disciplined inquiry offers a distinction between these five aspects of the research process: assumptions adopted towards truth, reality, knowledge, and how knowledge is to be used choices with respect to how disciplined inquiry is to proceed procedures for the collection of data techniques for the analysis of data interpretation of the findings with respect to previous research, knowledge and paradigm; critique of design/methods/analysis; dissemination of findings HiledRethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 24 paradigm that is the right one, or the wrong one, and many different paradigms can exist alongside each other. Some positivist scientistswill not be very happy with this, but I think that is simply a problem for them, and it is not my problem as such. It also needs to be stressedthat the notion of paradigm being used here is not that different from Kuhn's (1970) use of the term, but we will not be concerned with his arguments concerningscientific revolutionshere. Guba & Lincoln helpfully outline three areas of questions that all paradigms ultimately must be concerned with. These are: (i) ontological - concerned with the notion of what actuality exists, the nature of reality, and what can be known about it; (ii) epistemological- concernedwith the search for the foundations of human knowledge which can offer some assuredness of the truth of our knowledge claims; (iii) methodological- concernedwith establishingprocedures by which we can go about making additions to knowledge. These three areas are not completely independent and will place constraintson each other. In their scheme, Guba & Lincoln offer four major inquiry paradigms: positivism,postpositivism, critical theory and constmctionist,to which I would like to add at least one furtherparadigm - human science, which despite some areas of overlap does not easily fall into any one of the other four. The human science paradigm predates many of the cognate disciplines that have emerged over the past century or so, and deserves a fuller recognition than it at present receives. It is a paradigm of inquirywithin which such areas as phenomenological psychology, transpersonal psychology and discursive psychology could fit more comfortably.I will be spelling out my reasons more clearly for this proposal when we get to lectures 2 and 3. Strategiesof inquiry I have already described strategies of inquiry as concernedwith the issues of research design, providing an important bridge between the paradigm of inquiry on the one hand and the methods of data collection and analysis on the other. Hiles/ RethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 25 In considering strategiesof inquiry, I want to stress the importance of how exactlythe research question is formulated. Small and subtle changes in the wording of the research question can lead to widely different strategies and methods of inquiry. Choices need to be made between a hypothesis-testing approach, or a more data-driven,grounded theory approach. Choices need to be made with respect to acknowledgingand dealing with potential bias, with samplingthe phenomena under study, and with how the phenomena are to be studied, possibly selectinginore than one approach in a strategy of triangulation.It is possible to select any one of a number of different approaches that emphasizethe experiential dimensions of the participants and the researcher, together with adopting an attitude of co-operative inquiy. One matter that cannot be overlooked in all human research is the range of ethical issues that are raised. Whllst the highest standards of concern with respect to thepotential for harm, confidentiality,informed consent, and the uses to which the research will beput are required, it is necessary tonote that these can receive significantly different treatment within different paradigms of inquiry. For example, a number of specific considerations, especially with respect to confidentiality, must be given to the treatment of data that involves reports of individual experience, single case studies, transcripts of interviewmaterial, etc. Ethical principles developed in areas that have been largely concernedwith quantitative analysis, need to be completely rethought when a qualitative approach to data analysis is being considered. There is a strong case to be made for approachmg all participants as co-researchers.In addressingthe problems of confidentiality, I would argue that nothing less thanprocess consent should be considered for all huinan research, i.e. the ongoing monitoring of participants' consent to continue with taking part in the study. The standardmodel, with approval being sort from an ethics committee for human research, is woefully inadequate. UMethods of data collection Together with the traditional methods of experimentalresearchwith their inherent concernswith controlling variables, random assignment of subjects, and quantitative measurements, there is an increasing range of other ways of collecting data, such as interviewing, inquiry groups, single-case, lived inquiry, etc., usually associated with the qualitative approacl~.There is also the further matter which needs consideration, concerninghow in many areas Niles/ Rethinhirzg Paradigms/Lectu~.e1 -Page 26 of human research, the researcher cannot avoid being a participant in the inquiry process (Heron, 1970,1986,1996; Reason, 1988, 1994a, 1994b; Reason & Heron, 1995).Heron (1996) has called participatory research the fiifth inquiryparadigm. The point I wish to stress is that it is a mistake to think of these methods of data collectionsimply in quantitative vs. qualitativeterns. It is quite possible to obtain qualitative data under experimentalconditions, and perfectly feasible to obtain quantitative data from a range of interviews. Moreover, one research study can employ either one or severalmethods of data collection.The only constraints are those that follow from assumptions made by the paradigm of inquiry and the choices made with respect to strategiesof research design. ; j Data analysis i Here the distinction between quantitative and qualitative is probably of the i i most direct relevance. Nevertheless, the distinction shouldbe made only with caution, with the main considerationbeing whether the original i i paradigm of inquiry is consistent with the method of data analysis being J used. Quantitative analysis is of course a proven approach of enormousrange and application.But, there has been recently a number of developments, such as discourse analysis, narrative analysis, phenomenologicalanalysis, hermeneutics, etc., that can make the analysis of data that consists of meanings, accounts and descriptions far more effective.There will be a need to achowledge that the nature of interpretative techniques must inevitably involve the subjectivity and biases of the researcher/team involved in the data analysis.In some areas of research it may be possible to go a long way in eliminatingbias, while in other areas biases need to be worked with, rather than treated as somethingto be avoided at all cost (Janesick, 1994). Critical evaluation The final stage of any piece of research must be a critical evaluation of all that has been involved in the inquiry. A guiding principle in this respect should be a close examination of the claim that an addition to knowledge has been achieved. This will involve, in principle, three areas of reflection: Niles/ Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1 -Page 27 (i) reflection on the interpretationand implications of the findings; (ii) reflection on the strengths and weaknesses of the research design, methods of data collectionand analysis,the strategic choices made, possible sources of bias or unreliability in the data, and consequent limitations on the conclusions that can be made; (iii) thirdly, reflection involved in presentingthe findings to others in the wider community, who will in turn bring their critical powers of reflection to bear on the inquiry. In writing up, there is an obviousrequirement to review the existing literature from the widest variety of sources. The basic principle is the need to show how the research findings are an additionto knowledge and contributeto the topic area in some way. However, in the case of human scienceresearch, the model for writing-up an experimental study is not always appropriate. It is rather surprisingthat ths issue has received very little debate. With an experimental study, a review of the literature is necessary to formulate the hypothesis, but with an exploratory study (e.g, grounded theory) the findings cannotbe predicted, the theory emerges from the data. Clearly some discussion of the literaturemust be deferred until the discussion section of the report, when the findings are being presented and the grounded theory is being reported. The principle that must be adopted here is to include a literature review that reviews the general topic area, leading to a clear statement of the research question,and ajustification of theparadigm, strategies and methods of researchto be adopted. Then in the discussion section of the report a brief review of the areas of theory, knowledge and practice, that the findings have a bearing upon, can be included.The point here is simply that differentparadigms and strategies of research demand different approachesto writing-up. The style of writing for a human sciencereport is generally differentto the style used in the natural sciences.The style needs to be persuasive, argumentative,and possib1y employing a style that uses narrative devices, and subjective observations and expressions. Presentation of data can also raise different issues. Qualitative data does not yield easily to presentation in a summary table, etc. Extensiveuse of quotes and accountstaken from Hiled Rethinkir~gParadigm/ Lectrzre I -Page 28 interviewtranscripts may be necessary to illustratethe coding categories or emergent themes. The inclusionof a number of appendices of transcribed interviewsmay be required, to dojustice to the subtlety, sensitivityand underlying structure of the material. One approach might stress the need to interpret, reducing and synthesizingthe material down to its core meanings, while another approach might stress allowing the materialto stand on its own merits (e.g. elements, Ettling, Jenett & Shields, 1998). Critical evaluationwill necessarilyraise issues that question the data that has been collected. In the case of quantitative data, this invariably raises issues of validity, reliability and objectivity, but in the case of qualitative data a bfferent set of considerationswill come into play. Robson (1994) addresses this issue under the heading:- "Establishingthe trustworthinessof enquiry based on qualitative data". He proposes four criteria that need to be addressed with qualitative data: credibility, transferability, dependabilityand confirmability.These may discussed and established f?om a number of different perspectives, leavingthe reader of the published report to make their ownjudgment with respect to the trustworthiness of the findings. Three highlighted issues I intend to take up many of the issues raised by the model of disciplined inquiry in my two further lectures, but I would like to finish this first lecture with three issues that I think are worth highlighting. 0 Rethinking the comparison between quantitative and qualitativeinqlciry In Table 1.7,I have made a comparisonbetween the two broad traditions of the human and natural sciences,using the five stages of research.My purpose is simplyto reinforce how pointless it is to focus on the distinction between quantitativevs. qualitative.That is a distinctionwhich can only be meaningfulwith respect to the type of data and analysisbeing used. I think it is fairly clearthat when qualitative "methods" are being discussed, it is precisely the paradigm and the strategy issues, listed here under human science, that are being referred to, and not the type of dataper se. And, similarly,when quantitative "methods"are being discussed, it is those listed here under natural sciencethat are being referred to. While we only focus on the qualitativevs. quantitative distinction, the debate will always be confusing, and that probably suits the positivists rather well. It is much better to be comparing human scienceresearch with natural scienceresearch. However, it is necessary to recognise that this is only a comparison of two Hiles/ Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1-Page 29 TABLE 1.7 A comparison of the human and natural sciencetraditions Human Science Natural Science (Replacing the qualitative (Replacing the approach) quantitative approach) eterministicview of data driven; grounded method; variables are isolated & controlled; Method action research; case study; randomly selected experiential accounts; Isubjects; experimental & Analysis analysis; discursive, Iquantitative analysis; interpretative,hermeneutic, statistical description and persuasive, engaging, Hiles/RethinkingParadigrns/Lecture 1 -Page 30 broad traditions within the basic fkameworkof a general model of disciplinedinquiry. I think it is necessaryto stress againthat the human natural science distinction is far fkom new. It is at least as old as modern science, and in the case of human sciencethis is a tradition, in both Eastern and Western thinking, that goes back possibly severalthousand years. There is still clearly a place for recognisingthe key differencethat Ragin (1987) offers: " . .quantitativeresearchers work with afew variables andmany cases,whereas qualitativeresearchers rely on afew casesand many variables" (p. 22). However, the issue of number of cases and number of variables is not really an issue of quantitative vs. qualitative,but of the underlying paradigm and strategy of inquiry. There is clearly some value in distinguishingdifferent types of inquiry, and differenttraditions of inquiry, but this is best done within the model of disciplinedinquiry, whch emphasizes a broad continuum of assumptions, choices, etc. This comparison, in Table 1.7, raises a number of other issues concerning the nature of psychological inquiry. Two of these I will consider here, and several Wher issues will be taken up in my two other lectures. 0 Data-driven vs. Theory-drivenresearch The contrastbetween data-driven and theory-driven approaches to research is often expressed as the comparisonbetween a grounded theory approach and a hypothesis testing approach (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994; Strauss & Corbin, 1998;Denscornbe, 1998). Grounded theory is an approach that is inductive, and the emphasis is on discoveryrather than confirmation. The aim is to'develop a theory, or model of the phenomenon being investigated, that is demonstrably faithful to the actual lived experience of the people being studied.Its real value is in making explicitmuch of what is left implicit in research. It is an approachthat can be usefully applied to all methods of inquiry, but has largely been adopted by those working with qualitative data. Rather than startingwith a prediction, a theory, or a hypothesis to be tested, the researcher is encouragednot to review the literature in any depth, but to HiledRethinking Paradigms/Lecture 1 -Page 31 approach the research question with an open mind. This permits the theory to emerge fromthe data that is being collected, and hence this approach is oftencalled data-drivenresearch. The approach is not an excuse for loose and sloppy research, but requires acute phenomenological sensitivity, continual checking back to the original data, rigorous coding processes, a concentrationon active intentional processes, locating a single clear focus, leadingto a gradual refinement of theories and concepts. The issue of sampling also needs noting. The sample is not an apriori decision, but works more like following a trail of discovery.It is useful to think of this as involvingwhat is called theoretical sampling, and the sample can be regarded as complete when theoretical saturation is reached. o Triangulation There is the need to recognize that, with real world research and the grounded theory approach, multiple data sources can be explicitly drawn upon. In the real world, e.g. working in the field, in studying a single-case, or using an action research inquiry paradigm, collecting a variety of measurements and other sourcesmakes a lot of sense. Where it is planned that the theory will emerge from the data, it makes no senseto focus on a single variable, since it is clearly not consistentwith the logic of this approach to be able to predict which variables will be crucial to the findings. Ths approach is usually called triangulation,and although it is not limited to inquiry involving qualitative data, it is a key tool in that area. It basically involves trying to study a phenomena from several different angles or approaches, drawing on different methods, etc. But, triangulation is not limited only to data variables. Janesick (1994), drawing upon Denzin (1978), offers this useful list of five types of triangulation:data, investigator, theory,methodological and interdisciplinarytriangulation (see Table 1.8).While this list may not be exhaustive, it certainly indicatesthe many creativepossibilities of multiple theoreticalpositions, measures, methods, etc. Triangulation furtherpresents the opportunity of contributingsome crucial evidence towards claims of credibility and confirmabilityof the inquiry findings, i.e. in offering a much needed way of establishingcross-validation of the different sources, or perspectives, being used. Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 1 -Page 32 TABLE 1.8 Five basic types of triangulation Janesick (1994) lists five types of triangulation:'7 data triangulation: the use of a variety of data sources in a study 0 investigator triangulation: the use of several different researchers ord evaluators 0 theory,triangulation: the use of multiple perspectives to interpret a single set of data U methodological triangulation: the use of multiple methods to study a single problem . 0 interdisciplinary triangulation: drawing on the methods and insights of several disciplines HiledRethinkingParadigm/Lecture I -Page 33 For example, Robson (1994) points out that triangulation: " .. in its variousguises is an indispensable tool in real world enquiry. It isparticularly valuable in the analysis of qualitative data where the trustworthiness of the data is always a worry. Itprovides a means of testing one source of information against other sources. Both correspondences and discrepanciesare of value. lftwo sources give the same messages then, to some extent, they cross-validate each other. If there is a discrepancy, its investigationmay help in explaining the phenomenon of interest. [. . .]the by-products of triangulation are as useful as itsprimary purpose in validating information.It improves the quality of data and in consequence the accuracy offlndings. An alert~lessfor possible triangulation opportunities is a valuable quality in the enquirer. " @. 383) Summary In this lecture, I have tried to identify a major controversy in psychological inquiry, and propose a model of disciplined inquiry. The longer we focus only on the distinction between qualitative vs. quantitative research, then the longer will crucialparadigms of inquiry be lost to psychologicalresearch. The model I have proposed, I think, goes a long way towards overcoming the muddles in our thinking, and will put some of these many misleading controversies and arguments to rest. Most importantly of all, this model provides a guide to carryingout research that is clear, systematic and comprehensive. In my next lecture, I will take a much closer look at the human science paradigm, and focus on issues related to the growingfield of discursive methods of inquiry. And, in my final lecture, I will extend this further to phenomenological and transpersonal inquiry methods. Hiled Rethinking Paradignls/Lecture 1-Page 34 Lecture Selies. Brno, .Ap-ii 2091 Lecture 2: Paradigms Returned David Hiles De Montfort University, Leicester, UK. "Thereisno escaping thefact that thefindmental tension in the development of scientificpsychology is between the meaning of science and the meaningof being human." Amedeo Giorgi (1994) i%eIdea of Human Science. p. 90 "..human science requires a syncretic approachwhich integrates the results obtained through rnultischematic and multiparadigmaticsystems of inguiv. " Donald Polkinghorne (1983)Methodologyfor the HumanSciences. p. xi 0 Introduction In my fxst lecture, I outlined a model of disciplined inquiry that emphasized the need to address the issues relating to the paradigms that inform all research activity (see Figure 1.5).One feature of this model is that it rejects the simplisticcontroversies surroundingdifferenttypes, or approaches, to research. It furtherrejects the idea that the discipline of psychology must decide on a single paradigm for its research activities, but instead provides for the recognition of severalparadigms to coexist alongside each other, simply beginning from different assumptions. The quote by Giorgi at the beginning of this lecture highlights the key issue that I want to address here. In the development of our discipline, there has been a fundamental tension between the meaning of science and the meaning of being human. I would argue that the model of disciplined inquiry goes some way towards redefining the meaning of science, and reconciling Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 2 -Page 35 sciencewith the meaning of being human. Furthermore, Polkinghome's vision of multischematic and multiparadigmatic systems of inquiry can be fully realized within this model. Psychologyneeds to be positioned within the wider range of paradigms that it requires for its work. Indeed, we can move beyond the distractionof choosingbetween qualitative and quantitative methods, and instead face up to the real choice between a human science and a natural science paradigm that will informthe research process. This does not preclude other paradigms of inquiry from our consideration,indeed there is a case to be made for treating the human and natural scienceparadigms as broad traditions within which severalspecific inquiry paradigms can fit. Cment developmentsin psychology over the past ten years or so, suggest that the position I am advocating is urgently needed, if the perennial habit of margmalizing new fields of study is not to be repeated yet again. The proposal for two broad traditions of inquiry is not a new idea at all, but has been a recurring theme datingback to the work of the early pioneefsof psychological science. And as I have pointed out before, nor is the idea of human science very new either. In fact, as a method of human inquiry, it probably predates modern psychology by severalthousand years, and is the approach of choice in many Eastern approaches to psychology (Guenther & Kawamura, 1975;Bentz & Shapiro, 1998). For example, some of the exciting developments in psychology suggest that human science is far from marginal. The human science approach would clearlyembrace such contemporaryperspectives as social constructionism (Harre & Secord, 1972;Harre, 1979;Gergen, 1985,1999;Parker, 1992, 1998), culturalpsychology (Bruner, 1990;Hiles, 1996a), discursive psychology (Potter & Wetherell, 1987;Edwards and Potter, 1992;Harre & Gillett, 1994),and narrativepsychology (Bruner, 1986, 1990; Sarbin, 1986). The human science approach also would embrace established fields, such as humanistic-existentialpsychology (Fromm, 1949;Maslow, 1954; 1962; Rogers, 1961;May, 1958,1983; Frankl, 1962),phenomenological psychology (Giorgi, 1970, 1985, 1995; Spinelli, 1989;Valle & King, 1978; Valle & Halling, 1989; Valle, 1998)and counsellingpsychology (May 1939; Rogers, 1942, 1967; Clarkson, 1998).Together with recent developments in transpersonalpsychology (Braud & Anderson, 1998;Heron, 1998),and attemptsto rethinkpsychology (Smith, H a d , & Van Langhenhove 1995a, Niles/ Rethinking Paradignu/Lecture 2 -Page 36 1995b), there is an ovenvhelmingcase to be made for the centralplace that human science occupies in our discipline at present. But old ways die hard, and I am quite aware that many contributors to the above fields still cling to the natural science paradigm, and would be unhappy with being grouped with those who call themselves human scientists.Of course, that is their own decision. What I am sure of is that we no longer need to put up with the pointless squabble over qualitative vs. quantitative methods. Taking a serious look at the human scienceparadigm is the only way forward. Comparing the two major traditions of scientific research I have taken pains to stress that there is much more at stake in the different approaches to research than the simple contrast between qualitative and quantitativedata collection methods and analysis. In Table 2.1 there is a summary of the contrasts between the human science approach and the natural science (positivist)approach to research. It should be clear that there is much more at stake here thatjust a distinctionbetween qualitative and quantitative "methods". The human scienceparadigm recognizes that the explanation and understanding of human behaviour and experiencemust include accounts of subjectivelived experience, and that there is no search for one "truth" that overrides all other claims. Human action needs much more thanjust causal explanation,and needs also to be understood in terms of reasons, meanings, and the social and cultural contexts and practices that inform it. It is obvious that people respond not to events, but to the meaning of events. Human action displays intentionality and normativity. Our actionsreflect our beliefs, commitments and desires, and are governed by rules, standardsand conventionsand not simply by antecedent stimulus conditions. An emphasis on human behaviour needs to be made, which stressesthe goal-directed and purposeful qualities of human action. Human reflectivity, consciousness, experience and spirituality need to be valued. It may often be impossible to generatehypotheses concerning human actions and experiencesthat can then be tested under controlled laboratory conditions. It is perfectly feasible to approach phenomena with an exploratory attitude, so that theoretical constructs emerge out of the data collected. It is the uniqueness of human experiencethat needs to be respected. It is simply a fallacy that episodes Hiles/ RethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 37 TABLE 2.1 Two major traditions of scientific research (usually identified as qualitative Subjectivity -there is only lived Objectivity -research is the pursuit experience;inquiry is never free of objective truth; reality is lawfbl; fiom bias, never ideologically science involves a set of procedures shared experience between Knowledge is constructed and is historically situated; it is local, holistic, context specific Knowledge is discovered; it is universal, reductionist, context fiee; it is regarded as the guarantor of progress I Meaning is the central concern; Causal explanation - mechanical, something we inhabit; the emphasis deterministic models is on understanding, narrative I accounts, etc. Grounded theory -theories are Hypothesis driven - predictive; laboratory study, isolation and control of variables; experimental form of descriptions, discourse, Interpretation- hermeneutics; Statistical analysis - averaged data; methods adapted as necessary ide scheme of inferential . HiledRethinljng Paradigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 38 recounted in clinical case material (e.g. experience of loss of a loved person/object) can be explained by universal laws. Sincelived experience is unique, it can only be understood within an interpretative, hemeneutic framework. This perspective of the human science approach to psychological inquiry does align it with the concerns and methods of the other human and social sciences, which would include disciplines such as: semiotics,linguistics, anthropology, sociology, history, political science, philosophy, etc. Many researchers from these allied disciplines may be quite surprised atjust how many difficulties psychology makes for itself out of these issues. , On the other hand, the natural scienceparadigm takes a positivist stance. It places emphasis on the pursuit of somekind of "objectivetruth", on contextfree knowledge, on causal deterministic explanations, and the notion of the centralrole played by quantitative measurement. Conflict between competingtheories is resolved by a research enterprisethat emphasizes the testing of hypotheses, leadingto the rejection of one theory and the acceptance of another. While Table 2.1 does hghlight the considerable differencesbetween these two approaches to research, this can also be somewhat misleading. As I have stressedbefore, despite these differences, research can often combine both traditions, e.g. research can combine both quantitative and qualitative methods in a single study.Moreover, the contrast between the two approaches may be more of a continuum than the dichotomy that is presented here, or is usually appreciated in discussions on these issues. What is being stressed in Table 2.1 is that any comparison obviously involves far more than a difference in the use of measurement. It is the ontological, epistemological and methodological issues (cf. Guba & Lincoln, 1994)of the underlying paradigms that are at the core of any useful comparison to be made. A critique of the natural science paradigm Of course, the natural science approach, with its emphasis on quantitative measurements, statisticaltechniques and laboratory methods, has a well proven track record in psychology and all scientific disciplines. However the limitations are also all too obvious. A moment's reflectionwill lead to the realization that measurements are basically a means for categorizing potentially complex observations into a form suitable for mathematical Hiles/ Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 39 manipulation, and subsequent hypothesis testing. Such procedures always involvethe removal of context. It should be pretty obvious that context cannotreally be measured. So, if you can not measure it, do not study it, is the usual rationale. But the study of context is probably the most crucial contribution to understandingalmost any aspect of human action and experience! Measurement inevitably involves data loss and reduction. The common practice of averaging data collected fiom several subjectsfails to preserve any rneaningfbl record of the uniqueness of the individual. Typically, measurements and methods of data collection are devised to suit some apriori hypothesis, while other possible measurements and variables are completely ignored. Somethings may be very difficultto measure, so the scientist simply ends up studyingwhat it is easy to study. The simpler issues are examined at the expense of the more complex. Important areas of research are overlooked or deferred. Nevertheless, quantitativemethods do have a place in psychology. Indeed, because of their ease of use, and the influence fiom other disciplines, they are possibly still the most widely used approach, and they are fully accommodated within the proposed model of disciplinedinquiry.However, the point that must be made is that in such areas of research that are concerned especially with the situated and occasioned context of human action, with the subjective constructions of meaning and experience, and with transpersonal experiences, etc., then the l~ypothesis-drivenmethods of the natural science approach are far from appropriate. The human science paradigm The discipline of psychology is now in real danger of seriously lagging behind the other disciplinesof the human and social sciencesby not embracingthe human scienceparadigm. The selectionof one paradigm rather than another has a major influence on the type of research questions that psychologists can investigate, and on which areas of human behaviour and experience can receive serious study. Moustakas (1994) offers a very useful outline of what he sees as the common features of the human science approach (Table 2.2), which help in definingthe field. Hiles/ Rethinking Paradignu/Lecture 2 -Page 40 TABLE 2.2 Clark Moustakas - the common features of human science research Hiles/Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 41 An important set of considerationsthat operate when choosing to work within the human scienceapproach are presented below. Any one of these might be ajustification for adoptingthis approach: (i) the paradigm/perspective being adopted (ii) a research question that focuses on an exploratory and/or descriptive approach (iii) the emphasis on meaningrather than measurement (vi) a focus on subjectiveexperience rather than behaviour (v) the need to respect the uniqueness and context of the data collected. m l e I reject the simplistic distinctionbetween quantitative and qualitative research, I stillthink it is sometimesuseful to identify a broad tradition of inquiry that can be described as qualitative. It is an approachto research that does recognise human behaviour as complex, and emphasises that people respond not to events but to the meaning of events. The explanation and understanding of human actionsrequires more than simple causal, deterministicmechanisms. Human action and experience are the consequence of multi-determined factors, historically and culturally embedded. This approach is reflected in the emphasis found in the work of Polkinghome (1988), Moustakas (1990, 1994),Janesick (1994), Heron (1996, 1998),Reason (1994a) and many, many others. UBroadening the scope of psychological inquiry The basic idea behind the model of disciplined inquiry is of course to broaden the scope of psychological research. However, it needs to be acknowledgedthat there are a few approachesto inquiry that have been more or less adopted by main streampsychology over the years, even though they do not fit too comfortably within the natural science approach. In most casesthey are not even very easy to define in terms of the qualitative and qualitative distinction.They do however fit easily into the model of disciplined inquiry, and really are best considered within the human science approach. I will considerjust thee of these here. HiledRethirzhingParadignrs/ Lec&re 2 -Page 42 0 TheSingle-CaseStudy The case study is a very well establishedmethod of inquiry in educational and clinicalresearch (Robson, 1993;Denzin & Lincoln, 1994;Denscornbe, 1998).It is a method of inquiry that offersmany advantages: (i) focus on one instance -focussing upon a single instance, or a small number of instances of the phenomena to be investigated, canbring to light important insights that may have much wider implications the aim here is similar to the groundedtheory approach, to illuminate the general by looking at the particular; (ii) thick description -the single-case offers the opportunity to study a phenomenon in-depth, with considerable detail and with the emphasis on a thorough description - this is somethingthat a survey or a groupstudy approachmight preclude; (iii) focus on context and situational processes -human phenomena are embedded in complex social-cultural contexts and processes - the single-case offers a holistic approach that has a better chance in talung into accountthe full context that would be lost in a larger scale group study; (iv) real world setting -the single-caseis usually a naturally occurring phenomena, in contrastto laboratory experimentswhich are often contrived studies; (v) multiple data sources/methods -the single-case studyusually encourages the use of triangulation, i.e. a variety of sources and methods. As I have already said, the single-case study is generallywell accepted in psychologicalresearch, but it hardly fits in well with the a natural science paradigm, and neither is the distinctionbetween qualitative and quantitative research very relevant. 0 Action research The tern action researchwas coined by the social psychologist, Kurt Lewin (1952), and involves a spiral of cycles involvingplanning, acting, observing and reyecting (Robson, 1993; Denscornbe, 1998).Action research is HiledRethinkingParadigms/Lecture 2 -Page 43 concerned with practical issues that arise in a specific situation,by examining a particular practice and the resulting changes that follow from it. The aims of such research might involve the improvement of the practice, deeper understanding of the practice, or a clearer view of the situation in which the practice takes place. This type of research necessarily requires participation andlor close collaborationbetween researchers and practitioners, and is closely allied with the scientist-practitioner model of research (see for example, Robson, 1993).This approachto research is commonly used in such areas as educational, clinical, counselling and organizationalpsychology. It is an approach that has been considerably extendedby co-operative inquiry and participatory inquiry. o Co-operativeinquiry Although co-operative inquiry has been implicit in a lot of research in the human sciences since its inception, it is only recently that it has been spelled out explicitly (Heron, 1981,1996, 1998;Reason, 1994a; 1994b).It has affinitieswith action research and experientialresearch, and was obscured a little behind the banner of "new paradigm" research in the early 1980's (Reason and Rowan, 1981). Heron (1996) defines co-operative inquiry as involving: ". . two or morepeople researching a topic through their own experience of it, using a series o f cycles in which they move between this experience and reflecting together on it - eachperson is cosubject in the experiencephases and co-researcher in the reflection phases" (p.1). "Itis a vision ofpersons in reciprocal relation using thefull range of their sensibilities to inquire together into any aspect of the human condition with which the transparent body-mind can engage"Op. 1). Heron argues that in the traditional models of research, the roles of the researcher and subject($ are mutually exclusive, the researcher contributes the thinking that goes into the project, and the subject contributes the "data" for the study. But in the co-operative inquiry model, both contribute equally to the design of the research, and equally share in the experience. This is not research on people, but research with people. The emphasis here is that Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Leciirre 2 -Page 44 TABLE 2.3 John Heron - Co-operativeinquiry Heron (1996) outlinesfour phases that are involved in what is essentially a cyclic process: Phase 1: reflection on the topic of inquiry, choosing a plan of action, contracting Phase 2: a first actionphase of engagement E Phase 3: full immersion in the action phase E Phase 4: a second reflection phase, review, sharing data, plan for next actionphase, etc. And Heron has distinguishedfour kinds of inquiry outcome: CI transformations of personal being 0 presentations of insight, e.g. using creative expression 0 propositional reports of what has been explored, the outcomes and methods 0 practical skills associated with transformative action, andlor participative knowing HiZes/Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 45 research is a co-operative venture, in which the researcher plays a facilitatingrole, and all other participants should be seen as co-researchers. Heron has outlined four basic phases involved in this form of inquiry, and also spells out four kinds of inquiry outcome (see Table 2.3). It is interesting to note how pointless it would be to quantify the inquiry outcomes that Heronpresents here. Sincethey involve transformations of personal being, creative expression of insight, propositional reports and practical skills, they can only be effectively studied using human sciencetechniques. The notion of co-operative inquiry is closely related to the notions of participative inquiry, and human inquiry groups. There are also obvious l~nkshere with approaches such as heuristic inquiry (Moustakas, 1990), and with mindful inquiry (Bentz & Shapiro, 1998),which we will examine more closely in Lecture 3. Clearly, co-operative inquiry incorporates not only a methodology, but also . a set of values. There are clear parallels here with the values inherent to the idea of the working alliance in counselling and psychotherapypractice, where, incidently, I see the working alliance as the embodimentof mindfulness. But the point here is that it does seem entirely appropriate that the methods of inqulry available to counsellors and therapists should quite properly parallel the therapeuticpractices that are the very focus of their research. Indeed, in any psychological practice where the presence and personhood of the psychologistplays an essential role, only methods of inquiry that accept the co-operative and participatory role of the researcher canbe seriously considered. A calculus for the human sciences In the final part of my lecture I want to explorethe possibility of a calculus for the human sciences. This is an idea I had a few years ago, and presented in a preliminary form in a paper I read to the 17*hInternational Human SciencesResearch Conference (Hiles, 1998). I will briefly summarizehere the model I proposed in that paper. In my fmal Lecture, I will be outlining how I have now extendedthe model further. The use of the word caZculus, may seem a little odd, because it tends to be associatedwith numbers and making calculations, which would not fit too Hiles/ Rethinking Paradignu/ Lecture 2 -Page 46 I well with the human scienceparadigm. But, I am using the word "calculus" in the sense of its dictionary definition as: Calculus-a set of systematic rules,procedures or methods used in derivingsome understanding of aphenomena. T This is precisely what I have tried to set out to formulate. And, I would argue, it is precisely what the human sciences are in desperate need of at this As the model of disciplinedinquiry indicates, human science covers a wide area, and is in danger of lacking cohesion and an obvious focus. As a startingpoint to bring about some kind of order, I will make a very simple distinction. I think that it is fairly clear that our knowledge of the "world", and our experience of it, comes to us in one of two fundamentalways: (i) as sense data, as phenomena, as experience i.e. Iam able to seentear/feeZ(etc) what is there (ii) as discourse, as interpretation, as a sociaVcultura1construction i.e. Iam being told what is there I do not have the time here to discuss all of the philosophical issues that this raises, but I think this basic distinctionis quite clear enough, and is exceedinglyuseful in making some progress quickly. However, it is important to note that, while it obviously makes sense to differentiate these two modes of knowing, and to study them separately, I also am of the mind that ultimately the two are inseparable. Clearly, whatI can see is there,will be heavily constrained by my language and culture, and whatIam told is there has little value without my sense of there being somethingthere in the firstplace. The calculus I propose builds on this fundamentaldistinction, leading to refining of a further set of distinctions, such that each of the various paradigms of inquiry, and related strategies and research designs can fall into their appropriate places. In some cases, theoretical constructs and models may be brought in fiom outside psychology. But, the result is that the human scienceparadigm can take on a clearly articulated structure. Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 47 PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY NATURAL SCIENCE PARADIGM I Figure 2.1 A calculus for the human scienceparadigm Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 2 -Page 48 The outline is presented in Figure.2.1, where the field of psychological inquiry is first split into two broad divisions:the human scienceparadigm and the natural scienceparadigm, a division that I have taken some pains to justify fairly extensively so far. In turn, the human scienceparadigm is divided into: thephenomenologicalcalculus, and the discursive calculus. This division reflects the need for a calculus of human experience, and a calculus of human action. Each will consist of two broad sets of rules, procedures and methods for understandingtheir basic areas of concern. These two broad sets of rules are not in competition with each other, but are complementary and must be able to mutually interact. The natural science paradigm, of course, has its own calculus, but that will not be our concern here. I also suggest that a similar, but possibly rather different scl~eme, operates througl~outthe disciplines that make up the other human sciences, but here we are concernedjust with psychology. Human experience and action are best understood when the meanings that inform them are grasped. But the human scienceshave had to develop without any adequate model of meaning. The proposed division into a phenomenological and discursive calculus is further warranted by the realizationthat the main concern of the human science paradigm should be a systematic study of meaning.The way fonvard that I am proposing argues that the study of meaning raises two broad sets of issues: (i) the role that meaning plays in human experience, in human awareness and consciousness, (ii) the role that meaning plays in human communication, in humanjoint action, and the creation and exchange of meanings that constitutes human society and culture. The first of these falls basically within the area ofphenomenological inquiry, and constitutes a calculus of human experience. The second falls within the area of discursive inquiy, and constitutes a calculus of human action.I will concentrate here on the discursive calculus, and will leave the phenomenologicalcalculus for discussionin my final lecture. Hiled RethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 2 -Page 49 0 The discursive calculus One of the most excitingrecent developments in psychology has variously been called the 2*dCognitive Revolution, or the discursive am.The importance of the discursive approach to psychology becomes clear when it is considered that discourse can be defined as any exchange of meanings. This exchange of meanings is clearlythe cornerstoneto an understanding of human action. As Harr6 & Steams(1995) point out, this approachinvolves: " . . the use of some new methods that are animated by one of the major contemporary theories of human action. This is thepoint of view that highlights discourse as the characteristicfeature of human life. [..] in one way or mother [thesenew methods . .] have opened up an aspect of the general conception of human beings as active, symbol-using creatures intentionallyengaged injoint projects" (p. 1). While I might dispute whether discourse is the characteristic feature of human life, or, more realistically a cl~aracteristicfeature, I do go along entirely with the main point that Hm6 & Stearns are making. Discursive psychology, together with the allied field of social constructionism, constitutes a new and distinct paradigm of inquiry, that stresses a particular view of human beings, and introducesmajor new methods of inquiry. This is what I have called the discursive calculus. It is not the calculus of the human sciences,but it is a major component of this paradigm. In my original paper (Hiles, 1998),I was mainly concerned with the outline of this discursive calculus, and this is presented inFigure 2.2. Its chief features are the division into: a theory of signfunction, and a theory of context. Signs are the carriers of meaning, and are used subjectto certain prescribed and proscribed contexts. This division seemsjustified by the simplifiedmodel of meaning that is illustratedin Figure 2.3. The scientific study of sign function is pretty well established,viz. semiotics, and I have discussedthe typology of sign function elsewhere (Hiles, 1997). Context, however, is another matter. The study of context, with some exceptions, is still in its infancy. Context is a key issue because the nature of signs varies iYom completely arbitrary meanings (artifice) to deeply motivated meanings (motzj).Arbitrary signs (e.g. language) are highly dependentupon explicit codes for their management. These codes relate to the selection (paradigmatic codes) and the combination (syntagmatic codes) of signs. The field of linguistics is more or less the study of such codes. Hiled Retlzinking Paradignu/ Lecture 2 -Page 50 Phenomenological Figure 2.2 The discursive calculus Hiled Rethinking ParadigmdLecture 2 -Page 51 Meaning g Figure 2.3 A simplified model of meaning Context signifier & signified sign to its usage: (i.e.typology of sign function: (i)codes - syntagmatic?; icon, index, artifice, & motif & paradigmaticl . 3 Hiles 1994, 1997) (ii) discursive context +-I Sign Note that the term "paradigmatic"(as designatingselectionfrom a category) is being used slightly differentlyhere to the way it is used in the model of disciplined inquiry (designating a set of assumptions).It is also the case that I motivated signs (e.g. visual cultural symbols) are subject to codes of I selectionand combination,but these codes are much less explicit. The fields of culturalpsychology and narrative psychology are especiallyrelevant here.I I In addition, all signs are subjectto rules, standardsand conventionsfor their use in the exchange of meanings. Ths forms the basis of humanjoint action, and is what I have called discursive context. The emphasishere is on understandinghuman behaviour as situated occasioned action (Edwards & Potter, 1992;Hiles, 1996b).This is possibly the major focus of much of current discursivepsychology research. Context is possibly one of the most important areas of research that human science needs to address. Human beings need to be very good at discerning context in almost every aspect of their daily lives. The fact that psychology has hardly devoted much attention to the study of context is a very serious and embarrassing admission. If we are to begin anywhere (see Table 2.2), then.itis helpful to breakdown discursive context into a four-fold scheme which stresseshow context arises through processes of social construction, cultural embeddedness, conscious and unconsciousmotivation,and the spiritual tensions inherentto hukm existence. A specificresearch interest of mine is the way in which people, faced with a lack of context for a particular event or situation,will adopt or invent a context of their own by using narratives Wles, 1994b, 1996b, 1996~). There is a curious property of narratives pointed out by Watzlawick, Beavin & Jackson (1967) that is relevant here. What they have more or less characterize as third-orderknowledge correspondsto narrative structuresI have been studying. Watzlawick et a1suggest that: "..there is a strong reason to believe that it is really quite irrelevant what this third-orderknowledge of the world consists oJ;as long as it oflers a rneaningjidpremisefor one's existence" (p. 261). Human beings need context. When context is not available, they seem to invent it for themselves. It little matters what the resulting context is, as long as the context is made available in some way. The work of Sarbin (1986), Polkinghome (1988), and Bruner (1986, 1990)is especially relevant here. Hiled Rethinking Paradigm/ Lecture 2 -Page 53 TABLE 2.4 Discursivecontext: a four-fold division Situational Social Construction Cultural Culturally - Embedded ' preceding andfollowing event sequences; rules, expectations,protocols, social structures, discursive rules, etc. his.torica1,institutionsEized expressions of conscious and unconscious themes; rituals, ceremonies, myths, legends, etc.- . memories, identity, Personal Consciousl Unconscious Motivation personal constructs and narratives; creative imagination;projections, transferences, anxieties, feelings, defences, etc. Transpersonal Spiritual Tension discernment of existential givens, collective themes and concerns, mysteries, archetypal qualities, etc. Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 2 -Page 54 I thinkit should be clearthat the main emphasis of my proposed calculus for the human sciencesis inclusiveness. I am not interestedin developing a scheme that sets out to exclude useful and innovatory ways of inquiring into the human realm. Nor am I interested in a bewildering ragbag of theories and models and methods of research that offerthemselves either to misuse or likely obscurity. My search for a calculus for the human sciencesis designedto bring order and structureto want Ithink are the most important I issues that our discipline now faces. I will finish with two brief illustrative I , I examples of how this emerging scheme can be of help. I Two illustrative examples 0 TensionsbetweenData Analysis andParadigrz Assumptions II By far the most common form of inquiry involving qualitative data i collection involves interviewing.As a method of data collection, the i processes of structured and semi-structuredinterviewing are certainly a I I worthy topic for study (Breakwell, 1995; Mishler, 1986;Robson, 1993; Smith, 1995).In using interviewing as a method of data collection, and in 1 I I analyzing interview data it well worth bearing in mind Mishler's (1986) description of interviewingas "thejoint construction of meaning". Interviewing will usually involve tape recording, followed by transcription, and then analysis into themes, codes, etc. What I am mainly concernwith ! here is the analysis of interview data. There are severalapproachesto this 1 which come under the general heading of discourse analysis @urr, 1995; ! t j Gill, 1996;Potter, 1996). The choice of approachto discourse analysisis crucial when considered in the context of theparadigm of research being used (Hiles, 1999a,2000a). Many approachesto discourse analysis are derived from a social constructionist perspectivethat makes various paradigmatic assumptions which may not be compatiblewith the perspective to be taken. This may especially be the case in research in health psychology, counselling, psychotherapy, etc. When I first began to use discourse analysisto analyze interview data I had collected in connectionwith people's attraction towards the helping professions, I rather blindly adopted the recognized approachthat had been adopted within socialpsychology. These analyticaltechniques were earned Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 2 -Page 55 within a rather extreme form of social contructionism,that saw people's actions, and the accountspeople gave of their actions, as more or less the product of their discursive or linguistic environments. I have subsequently come to see such a perspective as little more than a thinly veiled behaviourism (Hiles, 1997d),but at the time I simply felt only disappointmentfor the outcomes of the analysis. The research question I had fiarned for my researchwas explicitlyconcerned with the lived experiences people inthe helpingprofessions could relate abouttheir choice of work. It was as a result of this paradigm clash, that Ibegan eventually to develop the model of disciplined inquiry. Recently, I have begun to use a slightlydifferent approachto discourse analysis that seems to allow a form of analysisthat facilitatesthe type of inquiry I wish to undertake. Thisparticular approach is calledInterpretive Phenomeno2ogicalAnalysis (IPA), and was developedby Smith,Jarman, & Osborn (1999). They point out that discourse analysis: ". . as generally conceived of in contemporarysocialpsychology, is sceptical of thepossibility of mapping verbal reports on to underlying cognitions [. .discourse analysis] regards verbal reports as behaviours in their own right which should be thefocus offunctional analysis. IPA by contrast is concerned with cognitions, that is, with understanding what theparticular respondent thinks or believes about the topic under discussion. Thus,IPA, while recognizing that a person 'sthoughts are not transparentlyavailablefrom, for exanzple, interview transcripts,engages in the analyticprocess in order, hopefully, to be able to say something about that thinking" (p. 219). The point that I specifically wish to make is that data analysis tools are not independent of paradigmatic assumptions.Indeed, the casual adoption of qualitative methods is a fairly pointless exercisewithout a considerationof the underlying paradigm(s) being used. The model of disciplined inquiry, and the related calculus for the human sciencesis designedto make such considerations explicit and straightfonvard. 0 Theorisinghuman differences For my second example, I would like to examine an issue that particularly illustratesthe importance of meaning in understanding human actions. I will draw upon another research interest of mine - human dzfferences. This is a Hiled RetIrinL~ngParadigms/Lecture2 -Page 56 very well establishedtopic within psychology, but this has largely been framed within the paradigm of psychometrics -the measurement of human differences.I will not enter into a critique of psychometrics here, but I do want to examine how a human science approachto human differences could make a contributionto this important area. At this present time, I am not aware of anyone else who has attempted to do this, but the issues raised seem to suggestthat a human science approach is long overdue. My own understandingof human differenceshas been strongly influenced by the British sociologist and cultural theorist, StuartHall. In a lecture that he delivered at Goldsmith's College, London, in 1996,Hall argues that human differences should be approached from a discursiveperspective. Ha11(1996) specificallyconcerns himself with racial differences, i.e. he focusses onjust one of the great classificatorysystems of differencewhich operate in human society (e.g. Gender/Class/Ability/Race/Sexuality/Age). His proposal is that race is a discursive category, and should be recognised as afloating signzfler. It is the meanings given to racial differencesthat matters, not the differences themselves, and meanings are not fixed, but float and slide, dependingupon discursive context. Of course, the reality of human diversity in terms of physical, social and psychological differences (what Michel Foucault means when he talks about the extra discursive) is not being denied. What matters are the systems of thought and language we use to make sense of these differences. When human differences become organized within language, within discourse,within systems of meaning, then these differences take on specificmeanings, and become a factor in the discursive organization of human action. The word "discursive" is used here to mark the transition theoretically, from a more formal understanding of difference,to an understanding of how ideas and knowledge of various differencesinform human actions and become embedded in various social and cultural practices. Hall's argument clearly generalizesto every category of human difference. It offers a model of human differences, in terms of how they are used in human interaction, which proposes that human differences actually work like a language. It is my believe that this insight could provide a basis for a completelyrevised programme of research into human differencesthat could be approached from within the human scienceparadigm of inquiry. Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Lecture 2 -Page 57 17Summary In this lecture, Ihave argued for the return of the human scienceparadigm, and the recognition of its righthl place alongsidethe natural science paradigm in psychology. This is designed to offer a considerably wider range of research approachesfor psychology to explore, together with a more systematicway of placing existing and emerginginquiry paradigms into somekind of general model of inquiry. I have also proposed a calculus for the human sciences designedto bring this potentially confusing field into some kind of order and structure, and I have explored the discursive calculus here in some detail. In the next lecture, I will explore the phenomenological calculus a little further, and I will try to drawtogether these threads to make some overall conclusions. HileslRetlrinking Paradigms/Lecture 2 -Page 58 :Lecture Series, Bmo, April 2001 Lecture 3: Paradigms Discerned David Hiles De Montfort University, Leicester, UK. "Manyof the most signzpcant and exciting lij5e events and extraordinary experiences-moments of clarity, illz~mination,and healing-have been systematical&excludedfiom conventional research." William Braud & Rosemarie Anderson (1998) TranspersonalResearchMethohfor the Social Sciences. p. 3 "Humanscience seeks to know the realitywhich isparticularly our own, the reality of our experience,actions, and expressions. Thisrealm is closest to us,yet it is most resistant to our attempt tograsp itwith understanding. Because of the success we have had knowing the world around us,the human realm has expanded itspower to such an extent thatwe can act to create wellbeing and physical security and comfort and to inflictuntold suffering and desiruction. Serious and rigorousre-searchingof the humanrealm isrequired " Donald Polkinghorne (1983)Methodologyfor theHuman Sciences. p. 280-1 Introduction What I tried to do in my first two lectures was to give some idea of the scope of psychological inquiry through a model of disciplined inquiry. I then tried to develop that scopeby spellingout what would be involved in a hurnan science approach. Specifically,I examinedwhat I have called the discursive calculus, with its particular emphasis on meaning, and the social/cultural context in which human actions are situated. In this final lecture, I want to examinemore closely thephenomenological calculus,which constitutesthe secondmain concern of the human scienceparadigm. Hiled Rethinking Paradignu/ Lecture 3 -Page 59 By the phenomenological calculus I mean that area of the human science approachwhich concerns itself with the exploration and systematic study of human experience, and the particularrole played by meaning in the wide diversity of experiences that the human being is able to report. This approach is by no means a recent developmentin psychology. For example, the field of phenomenologicalpsychology is well established, although not necessarily seen as mainstream, at least in the Western tradition. Indeed, it could be arguedthat this field has a much longer history than modem psychology itself, and has its roots in many ancient traditions and practices, across a wide range of cultures. My focus here will be very much on the recent upsurge of interest that has been sllown inphenomenological inquiy. We will examine a number of emergingnew paradigms of inquiry in psychology, especially in the rapidly expandingfield of transpersonal psychology. However, there is one other matter that I want to raise in relation to this general field of interest, and this is reflected in the two quotes I have used at the start of this lecture. Firstly, Braud & Anderson (1998) express very clearly a view held by myself, and many of my colleagues.For far too long, psychology has systematicallyexcluded areas of human action and experience,that some psychologists think are too difficult to study, or even are not suitable topics of study,that many other psychologists are now claimingto be much too important to be ignored. Problems of measurement, and lack of appropriate methods of inquiry, have often been the stock excuses offered for the exclusion of these areas of study. I want to argue that such excuses are no longer valid, and we shouldbegin to take seriously, the research findings that are beginning to be made available in these areas. Polkinghorne's (1983) plea for a serious and rigorous "re-searchingof the hu~?zanrealm" is in exactly the same vein. He points out that although this realm is closest to us, in the sensethat it concerns our direct human experience,it can be most resistant to careful study. But he also warns that the imbalancebetween our efforts in the natural science approach, at the expense of l-lmanscience, may be a contributoryfactorin the untold sufferingand destruction that seems to be ever present in the modern psyche. I remember that Carl Jung expressed very much the same idea when he observed that "thefuture of humankind is held by a single thread, the human psyche. "There is no more urgent topic to research than the human realm of experience, action and expression, especially the significant and excitinglife events and the extraordinary experiences these can entail. Hiles/Retl~il~hingParadigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 60 Each research inquiry begins with the formulation of a research question, This questionbecomes refined in the context of both the assumptions that need to be made, and the claims of the additionto knowledge that hopefully will be the outcome of the inquiry. When these paradigmatic assumptions are not very explicit, and the strategies of design, data collectionand analysis are far from obvious, usually a process of discernmentwill follow. I first encountered the word "discernment" in the work of William Blake, and I am using it here to correspondto a reflective process of exploration and discovery leading to new conceptual, or theoretical, or practical distinctions that emerge f?om whatever is the focus of study or concern. I am fairly certain that discernment is an importanttype of knowledge, with a major significancein its contributionto our understandingof both the ancient traditions of self-examinationand modem psychology. Also, discernment is possibly a key activity in mimy of the human sciences, and in qualitativemethods that involve interpretative data analysis. In many ways, the model of disciplined inquiry, and the discursive and phenomenological calculus, are the outcome of a process of reflective discernment. And in the title of this third lecture, my implicationis that discernmentplays a key role in how new paradigms of inquiry emerge. New and innovative paradigms of inquiry are discerned, as our research questions become more ambitious, more refined, and more urgent. I will begin with a brief outline of the phenomenolog.lca1calculus. I will then examine and explore four types of inquiry that seemto be the most relevant for the direction in which we seem to be heading. I will then conclude with a small example taken from my own research. The phenomenologicalcalculus In my second lecture, I argued that human experience and action are best understood when the meanings that inform them are grasped. I proposed two broad sets of issues, the first of which is concernedwith: the role that meaningplays in human experience, in human awareness and consciousness. It is this set of issues that is the defining focus of the phenomenological calculus, i.e. a set of rules procedures and methods for the systematic study of human experience. Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/Lecture 3 -Page 61 The roots of the phenomenological approach within psychology come fiom the philosophical school ofphenomenology, which was foundedby Edmund Husserl, and developed furtherin the work of Martin Heidegger, Jean-Paul Sartre, and Maurice Merleau-Ponty, etc. Following Franz Brentano, Husserl realized that intentionalitywas the distinctive mark of the mind and not consciousness per se. Ido not simply think (andtherefore I am), but always think of something.Human thought is always intentional.The mind and its object are inextricablylinked, and consequentlyHusserl saw a method for overcoming Descartes' mind-body dualism (Husserl, 1929).Thus, the key assumption that follows fiom phenomenology is that human consciousnessis structuredby the principle of intentionality.A human thought always exists in relationship to a world of some kind. Basic to being human is the fact that we live in relationshipto a world (intentionality),to other persons (interpersonality), and other subjects (intersubjectivity). Clearly any psychology interested in the study of 'subjective,lived experience and consciousness cannot ignore the field of phenomenology, and the various issues it raises. In my ownresearch, I have spent considerable effort in trying to develop a model of human intersubjectivity (Hiles, 1997c) There is one respect in which I think the application of phenomenology to psychology differs fiom its place in philosophy. One of the tasks that Husserl sets himself, in developingphenomenology, was a task that has been a perennial concern of Western philosophy. How can we eliminate uncertainty and doubt fYom our knowledge, i.e. to reach a point of certainty, from where we can build again our knowledge fromthis base of certainty? While this question may be of direct relevance to the philosophical enterprise, I would argue it has only of indirectrelevance to psychology. Whatwe can be certain about is a question for philosophy, but what is the nature of human conscious experience, and what role does it play in human action, is the question for psychology. But, because conscious experience was central to Husserl's scheme, the insights and theoretical principles that follow fiom it are clearly of major consequence for psychology. Thebest way of dealingwith this issue is by establishinga perspective of, what I have called,phenomenological sensitivity,i.e. a concern and sensitivityto the role played by conscious experience in all human action. It then becomes clear that the search inphenomenology for some principle of certainty is only of marginal interestto psychology, at least at present. There Hiles/Retl~inCingParadigms/Lecture 3 -Page 62 is no implicationintended here of discountingthe obvious relevance of phenomenology to such areas as humanistic psychology, counselling and psychotherapy, however. With psychology gradually taking on some of the insights and principles of phenomenology, there has been a paralleled development of the phenomenologicalcalculus, which now includes a number of new and exciting inquiry paradigms. Together with the established approach of phenomenological inquiry,this includes:heuristic inquiry, transpersonal inquiry,and rnindfil inquiry (see Figure 3.1). These inquiry paradigms have clearly definingfeatures that make them distinct from each other, while permitting considerable overlap too. While these four inquiryparadigms do not exhaust the field, they are the ones I think are most worth highlighting. PhenomenologicalInquiry It is often remarked that phenomenology is as much a method as a particular school of philosophy. It therefore lends itself to the development of a calculus in the manner I have set out in the previous lecture. The notion of phenomenologicalinquiry as a distinct inquiryparadigm follows directly from this, and has been forcehlly promoted in the early work of Amedeo Giorgi (1970). The more recent developmentsin this approach are well covered by Colaizzi (1978), Heron (1981, 1996),Giorgi (1985, 1999, Pohghorne (1989), Spinelli (1989), Moustakas (1994) and Valle (1998). Phenomenological inquiry aims to explore human experienceby closely examining the descriptions of "phenomena" (conscious experiences), and uncoveringthe meanings embedded in those experiences.Descriptions are usually obtained through transcribed personal interviews, or the recorded experientialaccountof the researcher. The analysisproceeds by a continuous bracketing of presuppositions of the researcher and continuous reflection on meaning, and this leads to thephenornenological reduction. The paradigmatic assumptionsof this type of research are that human experiencesnot only can be studied, but are fundamentalto the nature of knowledge itself, as well as being a central component of all psychological processes and human actions.From a strategicperspective there is an emphasis on co-operative inquiry, and a grounded theory approach.Data is collected in a variety of ways, but typically tluough interview, personal accounts and narratives. Inpractice, the analysis is descriptive, and involves HiledRethinking Paradigms/Lecture 3 -Page 63 HUMAN SCIENCE Figure 3.1 The phenomenological calculus Hiles/Rethinhing Paradigms/Lecture 3 -Page 64 a cyclic process of reading and re-reading, careful interpretationof meanings, leadingto the searchfor invariances and patterns, breaking down the material into constituitivethemes. These themes constitutewhat is seen as an understanding of the structure of the experience.I have summarized this process in Table 3.1. There are very many variants of this analytical process which this summarytries to bring together (Colaizzi, 1978; Giorgi, 1985;Denzin, 1989;Polkinghome, 1989; Janesick, 1994; Moustakas, 1994; Heron, 1996; Smith et al, 1999).Throughoutthe process of interpretation and analysis a process of discernment and bracketing is involved. It is important to point out that, with the exception of work by Moustakas (1990) and Heron (1996, 1998),a serious examination and formal study of this process of discernmenthas yet to be undertaken. The interpretativeprocess is not without its difficultiesand problems, and, we must be critical of the claims for bracketing of presuppositions, as they are sometimes quite unrealistic. It is my position that all research relies on its chosen paradigm, and therefore presuppositions are unavoidable. We simply need to work out ways of workingwith them, and not ways of eliminatingthem. In order to avoid confusion, I also prefer to make a distinctionbetween phenomenologicalinquiry and heuristic inquiry, althoughI am aware this is not widely made at present. This distinction is based upon the contrast between working with data from the experiences of co-researchers as opposed to working with data from the researcher's own experience. I prefer to use phenomenologicalinquiry for the analysis of experiences of co-researchers, and heuristic inquiry for the analysis of the researcher's own experience. Heuristic Inquiry Heuristic inquiry was developedby Clark Moustakas (1990), and bares some strikingresemblances to the idea of lived inquiy developed by John Heron (1998). The heuristic inquiry paradigm is an adaptation of phenomenological inquiry but explicitly acknowledges the involvement of the researcher, to the extent that the lived experience of the researcher becomes the main focus of the research. Indeed,what is explicitlythe focus of the approachis the transformative effect of the inquiry approach, through a process of what I think can usefully be called discernment. Moustakas has identified a number of core concepts, and the seven basic phases involved in this approach, and these are set out in Table 3.2. HiledRethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 65 TABLE 3.1 The key stages of analysis in phenomenological inquiry +transcription -) holistic reading +extract significant statements *meaning statements +theme clusters I$ constituentthemes +comprehensive constituent themes +fmal comprehensive constituent themes 3 [returnto the co-researchers for feedback and Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Lecture 3 -Page 66 TABLE 3.2 HeuristicInquiry Moustakas (1990) has developed his own very demanding version of an approach which he calls heuristic inquiry. This identifies the following processes:M Concepts: Identify with the focus of the inquiry Self dialogue Tacit knowing Intuition Indwelling O Focussing Internal frame of reference W Phases: Initial engagement Immersion O Incubation Illumination Explication Creative synthesis U Validation of the heuristic research Hiles/Rethinking Paradigm/Lecture 3 -Page 67 Here is how Moustakas (1990) describes this unique approach to research.. He proposes that heuristic inqulry involves: " . .. aprocess of internal search through which one discovers the nature and meaning of experience and develops methods andprocedures for further investigation and analysis. The self of the researcher is present throughout theprocess and, while understandingthe phenomenon with increasing depth, the researcher also experiences growing self-awarenessand self-knowledge (p. 9). " . .. The heuristicprocess is a way of being informed, a way of knowing (p. 10). . .From the beginning, and throughoutan investigation, heuristic research involvesself-earch, self-dialogue, and se'discovery; the research question and the methodologyJlow out of inner awareness, meaning, and inspiration. WhenI consider an issue, problem, or question,I enter into itfully . . .I may challenge,confront, or even doubt my understandingof a human concern or issue;but when I persist in a disciplined and devoted wayI ultimately deepen my knowledge of thephenomenon . . I ampersonally involved. .I may be entranced by visions, images,and dreams that connect me to my quest. I may come into touch with new regions of nzyselJ;and discoverrevealing connections with others (p. 11). " . . .Essentially in the heuristicprocess, I am creating a story that portrays the qualities, meanings, and essences of universally unique experiences(p. 13) . . .In heuristic research the investigatormust have had a direct,personal encounter with thephenomergon being investigated. There must have been actual autobiographical connections (P 14). " . . .Heuristic inquiry is aprocess that begins with a question or problem which the researcher seeks to illuminate or answer. The question is one that has been apersonal challenge andpuzzlement in the search to understand one's selfand the world in which one lives. The heuristicprocess is autobiographic,yet with virtually every question that matterspersonally there is also a social -andperhaps universal signflcance (p. 15). There is clearly more involved in heuristic inquiry than the researcher simply analyzingtheir own experience, which could easily be seen as a Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Lecture 3 -Page 68 variation of phenomenologicalinquiry. What Moustakas seems to offer is the much wider context within which the researcher engages with the research question, examinestheir own experience amongst a number of other explorations, and follows this through with an awareness of the transformativeprocesses at work in the research enterprise. Transpersonal inquiry Transpersonalpsychology has been calledthe fourth force in psychology, and represents a distinct new paradigm within psychology, because its assumptions and presuppositions are so different. At the heart of the transpersonal approachto psychology is an attempt to place human life and experience in its widest possible context. I personally cannot see that it matters in the least whether different cultures and groups of people, at differenttimes in human history, have come up with very different visions of reality, of our place in it, and the associatedreligiouspractices that help give meaningto human existence. What does matter is the recognition by psychology of the role that transpersonal beliefs and practicesplay in peoples' lives, and the importantplace these can have in explaining and understanding an individual's experience, actions, growth and development. In Hiles (2000b), I describethe aim of transpersonalpsychology as to offer a synthesis of what may seem, on the surface, to be two quite different traditions - science and spiritualpractice, and what has emerged is a new field offering new approachesto psychotherapy, human development, crisis, etc. Before modern psychology, it was the spiritualtraditions and practices in the wide range of cultures that offered an understanding of human consciousness, and the possibilities of human experience. Transpersonal psychology can be seen as an attemptto bring the world's great spiritual traditions together with the basic ideas of (Western)modem psychology. This has, to a large extent, already been achieved in Easternpsychology, we arejust comingrather late to this in the West!! The vitality and enormous scope of this field is demonstrated, amongst many other examples, in the pioneering vision of Ken Wilber (1977, 1980, 1983, 1998,2000), in research on spiritual emergencejemergency and the model of the holotropicmind developed by StanislavGrof (1985, 1988, 1998; and with Christina Grof 1989, 1990), and in John Heron's (1998) proposal for a person-based, person-centred spirituality. Hiies/ RethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 69 TABLE 3.3 Braud & Anderson (1998, p. 256-83) - a general scheme ConventionalMethods 1.Experimental designs 2. Quasi-experimental designs 3. Single-subject designs 4. Action research 5. Correlationalapproaches 6. Causal-comparative studies 7. Naturalistic and field studies 8. Theoretical approach 9. Grounded theory 10.Historical and archival approaches 11.Content analysis, textual analysis and hermeneutics 12.Narrative and discourse analysis 13.Case studies and life stories 14.Interviews, questionnaires and surveys 15.Meta-analysis 16.Parapsychological assessments and design issues 17. Physiologicaland 1 biomedical 1.Phenomenological 1.Integral inquiry approach 2. Intuitive inquiry 2. Heuristic Research 3. Organicresearch 3. Feminist approaches 4. Transpersonal- 4. Experientialresearch Phenomenological method inquiry 5. Cooperative inquiry 5. Inquiry informed by 6. Participatoryresearch exceptionalhuman assessments and HiledRethinXingParadigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 70 While the transpersonalfield has been developing since the early 1970's, it has been more or less waiting for appropriateresearch methods in order to emerge into the mainstream. The very nature of the transpersonalparadigm, where the basic assumptions of the field are so differentfiom other areas of scientific inquiry, requires paradigms of inquiry that are necessarily quite different. Recently however, considerableprogress has been made in the development of research methods more appropriate to the paradigm of transpersonal inquiry.Braud & Anderson (1998), Heron (1998) and Valle (1998) have considerably extended the range of research methods that this area might adopt. These include lived inquiry, integral inquiry, intuitive inquiry, organic research, transpersonal-phenomenological inquiry, iinquiry informed by exceptional human experiences. Braud & Anderson's work in particular reflects the need for a systematic approach to the development of new inquiry paradigms. They also use the term disciplined inquiry to characterizethe research process, but do not offer a model in the way that I have done. They do draw up a general scheme which usefully tries to bring some order to the confusingdiversity of methods of data collection and analysis, which I have summarizedin Table 3.3. They propose five new methods for transpersonalresearch, which, although I would argue do not seem to differ significantlyfrom the intermediate approaches, are still useful variants of the inquiry process that will clearly extend the field. I have summarizedthese in Table 3.4. Mindful inquiry There is one last approach that falls within the phenomenological calculus rather well, and that is the refieshingly different approachto research called mindful inquiry (Bentz & Shapiro, 1998), which is summarized in Figure 3.2. Mindful inquiry is described as a synthesis of four intellectualtraditions: phenomenology, hermeneutics, critical social theory, and Buddhism. Scientificresearch is recognized as one of our many ways of knowing, and needs to be connected with the other ways. The emphasis in this approach is inplacing the inquirer at the centre, and research from this perspective is seen as intimately linked with the awareness and the experiencedworld of the researcher. Research can be seen to contribute explicitly to the transformation of the researcher's sense of self or identity. The idea of bringing mindfulness into disciplined inquiry is exciting, as it stresses focus, intention and awareness of whatever is present in a situationor experience. HiledRethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 71 TABLE 3.4 Braud & Anderson (1998) -five new transpersonal methods Brief outline of the approach Inquiry is multifacetedand pluralistic - and explores a research questionthat has great siMcance to both researcher and co-researchers - the nature of the research question determinesthe choice of methods, data sources, alternativeforms of knowing and ways of working with Tlis approach uses transpersonal slulls, such as intuition and alternative states of consciousness - essential This approach has grown fiom heuristic and feminist This approach has been developed by Ron Valle and EHEs are extremely rich in life-changing impacts, Hiled Rethinkring Paradip/ Lecture 3 -Page 72 Critical Social Phenomenology Science Mindful I Hermeneutics I I Figure 3.2 Mindful inquiry - four knowledge traditions pent2 & Shapiro, 1998, p. 38) Hiles/ Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 73 These are of course the essential qualities of all human inquiry. Some of the ideas involvedin this approach that are listed by Bent & Shapiro are given in Table 3.5. There is of course considerable overlap here with'some of the other inquiry paradigms that we have looked at. But the point is that we need to be open to these different possibilities, the different emphasis and different traditions. There is one aspect of mindful inquiry that I have special interest in, and that is the inclusion of critical social theory, which is also included in Guba & Lincoln's (1994) scheme (see Figure 1.5). I do not have time to develop ths point in these three lectures, but one of the features of critical theory that I think is most important is the recognition that theories, explanations and understanding can be empowering. One of the goals of psychological inquiry must be empowerment, i.e. the use of psychological knowledge to empower people to make informed choices, express themselves fieely, and challenge discrimination, oppression and unnecessary suffering. This is the expressed aim of such areas as feminist psychology (Nicolson, 1994), and much of discursive and criticalpsycllology (Parker, 1998). Some tensions arising in phenomenological inquiry Allfour approacheswithin the phenomenological calculus, which we have looked at, share one thing in common -the focus on the exploration and study of human experience. The study of human experienceis certainly not without its difficulties, not least of which is the sheerbreadth of experiences this would entail. As a final thought, I have tried to take a broad view of the nature of this field of studyby identifyinga number of tensions that seem to underly much of the research involved. Five of these tensions are presented in Table 3.6. There seems to a be a fundamental tension between experience that is grounded in human knowledge systems or in more everyday occurrences, and experience that is more subtle, involvingperhaps deeper or altered states of consciousness. This table reflects another thread of the phenomenological calculuswithin the human science approach. Human experience may be constructed, or principled by the culture we are embeddedin, or may reflect a more directly experienced "found" order in things. It may be spontaneous, or may result fi-omproven practices or rituals. It may be common and ordinary, or quite exceptional in nature. It may be intentional, or may involvetranscendent experience of the subjective experience of knowing itself. It may be grounded in the ego and self, or inay be transpersonal, beyond ego and self Hiled RethinkingParadigms/Lecture 3 -Page 74 TABLE 3.5 Mindful Inquiry Awareness of self and reality and their interaction is a positive value in itself and should be present in research processes. Tolerating and integrating multiple perspectives is valued. It is important to bracket our assumptions and look at the often unaware, deep layers of consciousness and unconsciousness that underlie them. All research involves both accepting bias - the bias.of one's own situation and context - and trying to transcend it. We are always immersed in and shapedby historical, social, economic, political, and cultural structures and constraints, and those structures and constraints usually have domination and oppression, and therefore suffering, built into them. Inquiry often involves the critique of existing values, social and personal illusions, and harrnfbl practices and institutions. Hiles/ Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 75 TABLE 3.6 Tensions in the phenomenological study of experience Hiled RethinLiciltgParadignd Lechwe 3 -Page 76 An illustrative example For anumber of years now, I have been engaged in a research project that could be said to have almost taken over my life. It began with some insights from the work of the 18" Century English poet and artist William Blake. These insights particularly related to understanding the processes of transformation that can be involved in the experience of human suffering.I especiallywanted to apply these insights in my practice as a counsellor and therapist, and also engage in research that would bring some form to these insights that could then be shared with others in the field. When this all started, I was not even that sure that what I was doing could evenbe called "research." In fact, it was only several years later that I discovered that what I had been doing all alongwas really heuristic inquiry, or perhaps even mindful inquiry. Coming across the work of Clark Moustakas (1990), I immediatelyrecognised the phases of engagement, immersion, incubation and illumination in my own work. I have reported some of the methodological issues involved in this work before (Kles, 1999b),and I only will briefly summarisehere some of the insights that I have gained about the nature of heuristic inquiry from my own direct experience in using it. (i) Heuristic inquiry is a research process that is difficultto set any clear boundaries to, with respect to duration and scope -it should not be undertaken lightly. (ii) In heuristic inquiry, the research question chooses you -the research question is usually deeplypersonal in origin. (iii) Heuristic inquiry highlights the importance of working with the heuristic process of others - especiallywith the historical recordings of previous inquiry - indeed, it turns out that the works of writers, poets, artists, spirtual leaders and scientistscan all be usefully treated as the creative products of heuristic inquiry -heuristic inquirywas probably the first research method adopted for psychological inquiry. In addition to my own self explorationand lived inquiry, I have also designed and carried out severalphenomenological/co-operativeinquiries. I have interviewed people who have claimed to have had a near-death experience, people who have been victims of serious crimes, and people who have been attractedto voluntary, or paid work, in ahelping role with others Niles/ RethinkingParadigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 77 (i.e. counsellors, carers, nurses, social workers, advocates, etc.). I will report onthe analysis of some data f?om this latter area. In Table 3.7, I have presented a shortpiece of transcriptionfkom a much longer interview with an adult male who has spent much of his later life in a helping role with other people. The research question behind this research is: to what extent does a life crisis, or an experience of suffering, lead to aperson choosing to be in a helping role with others? I can only illustrate the approachto analysis. I used IPA (Smith et al, 1999) because it is clear, straightforward and was certainly preferred over other approachesto discourse analysis, since it takes on a phenomenological approach. I approach an interview as a process that will involvethejoint construction of meaning. I try to keep an open mind, and simply try to facilitate the co-researcherin exploringtheir experience in the waithey feel most comfortable. Although the topic, that has been mutually agreed upon, is somethingthat I have proposed, I rarely have more than three or four questions that I expect to ask. The analysis involves a carell transcription, which is then arranged in a coluinn down the middle of a page, with wide margins to each side.Emphasis is coded by underlining, silences andpauses are indicated by squarebrackets [. .1.ldentrfiersare removed or replaced by neutral words. After reading and re-readingthe transcript several times, the margin notes can be made. The left-hand margin, is used for mytlzling that strikes me as significant or interesting.My own technique involves selectinguseful or apt phrases fiom the transcriptionthat seem important, and putting my own commentsin [square brackets]. The right-hand margin is used to note emergentthemes, and highlightingkey phrases that repeat, or seemto be most salient. The next stage would involve looking for connections, generatingtheme clusters, and then a list of constituent themes. The matter that I would like to highlight here is that althoughthe analysis seems to generate a productive set of basic themes fiom the original data, there is still a feeling of incompletenessin the analysis (Hiles, 2000a). This incompletenesscan be summarised as follows: (i) the process involved in the identificationof themes has been fairly well formulated,but this usually is at the expense of retaining some notion of context -and as I have pointed out before, context is a necessary constituent of meaning. Hiles/RetltirJLingParudigns/Lecture3 -Page 78 Table 3.7 Interview transcript - IPA analysis Hiled Rethinking Paradigms/ Lecture 3 -Page 79 (ii) one obvious aspect of context is narrative,and techniques of discourseanalysis and phenomenological analysis are woefully poor in analysing narrative. I have arguedpreviously for the'careful analysis of structure and topic changes in counsellingand interview transcripts (Hiles, 1996~). (iii) what is not said also createsproblems for analysis -the basic groundedtheory approachthat requires continuouslygoing back to the data - causesmuch difficulty with respect to silences, evasions, denials, etc. (I once observedthis in data that was not mine. An interviewwith a terminally ill patient, who kept up the position that they were unaware of their condition -P A seemed to fall far short of the subtle nuances of meaning inthat material). Each of these three issues is illustrated in the data in Table 3-7.Context is a continuing challengeto capture, the transcript and analysis illustrate two spontaneousnarrative accounts, which mirror the subtle topic change in the co-researcher's account (Hiles, 1996~).There are also a number of silences towards the end of this part of the transcriptthat have "meaning."The challenge is to extend the calculusto such issues, but this can only be done by first collecting the data, which then reveals the problems to be faced by researchers working in this field of inquiry. Summary I did not set out to persuade anyone to change their research methods, but insteadI have tried to make out a case for an openness to a wider range of methods of inquiry. I have tried to clarify some of the confusions and controversies that plague this area of debate. At the heart of my proposals is a model of disciplined inquiry that places the whole research process in a general context. I have argued that there is a place in psychological inquiry for a human science approach. This approachentails: an explorationof a wider range of paradigms a preparedness to work with qualitative data Hiles/ RethinJEingParadigms/Lectvre 3 -Page 80 consideringa wider range of research strategiesand methods of data collection a serious and systematic study of meaning the study of context the study of human experience a commitmentto examine and critique these paradigms of inquiry in order to refine and improve them I am certainthat psychology will never progress as a science without a commitmentto research, and the adoption of appropriatemethods of inquiry. And, I am fairly certain that psychologywill not progress much further with just the one recognised paradigm of inquiry. What is needed is a number of differentparadigms, with each committed to establishing its own criteria for being rigorous, systematic and convincing. I have pointed out the red herrings that I think we might be in danger of being mislead by, and I have offered a general model of disciplined inquiry within which we can all work alongside each other. I set out on my three tasks with the intention of erecting a few signposts, so that if you ever find yourself walking this way again, then you will be able, more or less, to find your way about. And that is what I hope you can discern that I have been able to achieve. Thankyou. HiledRethinking Paradigms/Lecture 3 -Page 81