26 Language and speech to our motor, articulator)/, habits. If this feedback control is disturbed, e.g. b, the imposition of an artificial delay upon our reception of our own speech, dis turbance in the production of our utterance is likely to result. Those who an born deaf or who become deaf before the acquisition of speech habits are rareli able to learn normal speech completely; similarly, a severe hearing loss later ij life is likely to lead eventually to a deterioration of speech, although not dowj to the same level as those born deaf. Cnapter 4 ■rj-ie description and classification of speech sounds Notes 1 Fant(1956). 2 Hollien & Shipp (1972); Russell et al. (1995). 3 Denes & Pinson (1993: lOlff). 4 For example, Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2011). 5 Abberton & Fourcin (1984). j, [Phonetic description \\" have considered briefly both the mechanism which produces speech sounds ami also some of the acoustic and auditory characteristics of the sounds themselves. We have seen that a speech sound has at least three stages available for eitigations—the production, transmission and reception stages. The most c ii venient and brief descriptive classification of speech sounds relies either on artieulatory criteria or on auditory judgements, or on a combination of both. 11 i>se sounds which are commonly known as consonants are most easily described mainly in terms of their articulation, whereas the description of vowels depends mure on auditory impressions. 4.1 Vowel and consonant Two types of meaning are associated with the terms vowel and consonant. In one type of definition consonants are those segments which, in a particular language, occur at the edges of syllables, while vowels are those which occur at the centre of syllables. So, in red, wed, dead, lead, said, the sounds represented by are consonants, while in beat, bit, bet, but, bought, the sounds v (represented by are vowels. This reference to the functioning of sounds in syllables in a particular language is a phonological definition. But once any attempt is made to define what sorts of sounds generally occur in these different syllable-positions, then we are moving to a phonetic definition. This type of definition might define vowels as median (air must escape over the middle of the tongue, thus excluding the lateral [1]), oral (air must escape through ...the mouth, thus excluding nasals like [n]), ffictionless (thus excluding fricatives like [s]), and continuant (thus excluding plosives like [p]); all sounds excluded from this definition would be consonants. But difficulties arise in English with this definition (and with others of this sort) because English /j,w,r/, which are consonants phonologically (functioning at the edges of syllables), are vowels phonetically. Because of this these sounds are often called semi-vowels. The reverse type of difficulty is encountered in words like sudden and little where 28 Language and speech the final consonants lal and HI form syllables on their own and hence must the centre of such syllables even though they are phonetically consonants, and even though /n/ and III more frequently occur at the edges of syllables, as jn net and let. When occurring in words like sudden and little, nasals and latera are called syllabic consonants. In this chapter we will be describing and classifying speech sounds phonetica: l\ (in the next chapter we return to the phonological definitions). We shall find that consonants can be voiced or voiceless and are most easily described who] in articulatory terms, since we can generally feel the contacts and movement!, involved. Vowels, on the other hand, are voiced and, depending as they do on subtle adjustments of the body of the tongue, are more easily described in tert-of auditory relationships. 4.3 Consonants For consonantal articulations, a description must provide answers to the following questions: (1) Is the airstream set in motion by the lungs or by some other meani. (pulmonic or non-pulmonic) (2) Is the airstream forced outwards or sucked inwards? (egressive or ingressiv , (3) Do the vocal cords vibrate or not? (voiced or voiceless) (4) Is the soft palate raised, directing the airstream wholly through the mouth, or lowered, allowing the passage of air through the nose? (oral, or nasal i nasalised) (5) At what point or points and between what organs does closure or narrowing take place? (place of articulation) (6) What is the type of closure or narrowing at the point of articulation? (mannei of articulation) In the case of the sound [z], occurring medially in the word easy, the following answers would be given: (1) pulmonic (2) egressive (3) voiced (4) oral (5) tongue blade-alveolar ridge (6) fricative. These answers provide a reasonably full but concise phonetic label for the sound a more detailed description would include additional information concerning, for instance, the shape of the remainder of the tongue, the relative position of tfr jaws and the lip position. j l Egrcs'>*e pulmonic consonants Description and classification of speech sounds 29 .. I, ,,wnds are made with egressive lung air. Virtually all English sounds p,'List ^ tf,e' exception being [p,t,k], which in some dialects sometimes l^X"^^ §4-3-9)- ( 3 ; Voicm? 1i,nt-i - if articulation, a consonantal articulation may involve the vibration A' u'' ■ I ords. i.e. may be voiceless or voiced, nl ■ * . 2 3 p/aco of articulation ief points of articulation are the following (reference is made to the -,i In.-* oi ..phrases): the companion website where tJie articulations can be seen in spoken Bilabial—The two lips are the primary articulators, e.g. [p,b,m]. (See videos I. 12. 5.18. 6.0, 6.14, 14.6.) Labiodental—The lower lip articulates with the upper teeth, e.g. [f,v], (See videos 3.13, 4.14, 10.14, 13.14.) Dental—The tongue tip and rims articulate with the upper teeth, e.g. [0,5], as ,i think and then. (See videos 6.24, 10.21, 12.16.) Ai veolar—Either the blade, or tip and blade, of the tongue articulates with the alveolar ridge, e.g. English [t,d,l,n,s,z], (See videos 3.1, 3.15,3.23, 5.16, 9.25, II. 10.) Post-alveolar—The tip of the tongue articulates with the rear part of the alveolar i idge, e.g. 14 as at the beginning of English red. (See videos 2.4, 4.2.) Rftroflex—The tip of the tongue is curled back to articulate with the part of the hard palate immediately behind the alveolar ridge, e.g. [.L] such as is found n South-West British and some American English pronunciations of red. PalatO-alveolar—Either the blade, or the tip and blade, of the tongue articulates vith the alveolar ridge and there is at the same time a raising of the front of He. tongue towards the hard palate, e.g. [J",3,f,d3] as in English ship, measure, beach, edge2 (See videos 1.1, 8.15, 9.1, 11.23.) Pai a:al The front of the tongue articulates with the hard palate, e.g. [j] or [9] as in queue [kjui] or [kcu:] or a very advanced type of [k,g] = [c,j], as in Trench quitter or guide. (See videos 5.5, 14.14.) Velar—The back of the tongue articulates with the soft palate, e.g. [k,g,rj], the List as in sing. (See videos 4.21, 7.1, 10.4, 11.1, 14.11, 15.22.) 1 ijlar—The back of the tongue articulates with the uvula, e.g. [k] as in French ■ ouge. "jlottal—An obstruction, or a narrowing causing friction but not vibration, between the vocal cords, e.g. English [h] as in ham. 30 Language and speech In the case of some consonantal sounds, there may be a secondary place of articulation in addition to the primary. Thus, in the so-called 'dark' [J], as at the end of bull (see video 1.20), in addition to the partial alveolar contact, there is an essential raising of the back of the tongue towards the velum (velarisation); or, again, some post-alveolar articulations of [j] are accompanied by slight lip-rounding (labialisation). The place of primary articulation is that of the greatest stricture, that which gives rise to the greatest obstruction, to the airflow. The secondary articulation exhibits a stricture of lesser rank. Where there are two co-extensive strictures of equal rank an example of double articulation results. 4.3.4 Manner of articulation The obstruction made by the organs may be total, intermittent, partial, or may merely constitute a narrowing sufficient to cause friction. The chief types of articulation, in decreasing degrees of closure, are as follows: (1) Complete Closure Plosive—A complete closure at some point in the vocal tract, behind which the air pressure builds up and can be released explosively, e.g. [p,b,t,d,k,g,?] as in pay, boot, tea, down, car, gate and in a Cockney pronunciation of water as [woctfa]. Apfricate—A complete closure at some point in the mouth, behind which the air pressure builds up but the separation of the organs is slow compared with that of a plosive, so that friction is a characteristic of the second part of the sound, e.g. [f,d3] in cheese, joke. Nasal—A complete closure at some point in the mouth but, the soft palate being lowered, the air escapes through the nose, e.g. [m,n,rj] as in modern, name, sing. These sounds are continuants and, in their (most usual) voiced form, have no noise component; they are, to this extent, vowel-like. (2) Intermittent Closure Trill (or Roll)—A series of rapid intermittent closures made by a flexible organ on a firmer surface, e.g. [r], where the tongue tip trills against Ihu alveolar ridge as in Spanish perro, or [r] where the uvula trills against the back of the tongue, as in a stage pronunciation of French rouge. Tap—A single tap made by a flexible organ on a firmer surface, e.g. [r] where the tongue tip taps once against the teeth ridge, as in many Scottish pronunciations of English M. (3) Partial Closure Lateral—A partial (but firm) closure is made at some point in the mouth, the airstream being allowed to escape on one or both sides of the contact. These sounds may be continuant and frictionless and therefore vowel-like (i.e. approximants in (5) below), as in [Li] as pronounced in the south of England little [M] or they may be accompanied by a little friction [j] as in fling or by considerable friction [1] as in please. (5) Description and classification of speech sounds 31 Harrowing Fricative—Two organs approximate to such an extent that the airstream passes between them with friction, e.g. [,p,f,v,M,e,a,s,z,r,3,c,x,h]. In the bilabial region, a distinction is to be made between those purely bilabial such as [,p] where the friction occurs between spread lips, and a labial-Velar sound like [av] where the friction occurs between rounded lips and is accompanied by a characteristic modification of the mouth cavity brought about by the raising of the back of the tongue towards the velum, [ and more close' e'g' wllile a pair of dots over \m'-'■!■ e-s- Ia!> indicates centralisation. The vowel quality mentioned above, that between C.[o| and C.[o] and centralised, can in this way be symbolised as b" 01 lSl - tt is, moreover, possible to give a visual representation of these vowel relationships on a chart which is based on the Cardinal Vowel tongue positions. The simplified diagram shown in Fig. 5 is obtained by plotting the highest point of tongue raising for each of the primary Cardinal Vowels and joining the points togtrhi 1 Die internal triangle, corresponding to the region of central or [a]-type ...^ur sounds- is made by dividing the top line into three approximately equal sections and drawing lines parallel to the two sides, so that they meet near the base or !he figure. On such a figure, the sound symbolised by [rj] or [o] may Have its relationship to the Cardinal scale shown visually (see the black circle on Fig. 5). I. rnust be understood that this diagram is a highly conventionalised one which shows, above all. quality relationships. Some attempt is, however, made to relate the shape of the figure to actual tongue positions: thus, the range of movement CI [i] O C.8 [u] Q C.7 [0]

(hose used for the Cardinal Vowels. However, on the IPA chart the unrounded vowel is always the first of the pair and the rounded the second; Front Central Back Close Close-mid Open-mid Open figure 7 Articulatory labels combined with the Cardinal Vowel diagram. •40 Language and speech this means that we cannot say that the first corresponds to the primary cardi and the second to the secondary cardinal. (It will be remembered that prim," , cardinals involve the most frequent lip positions, back vowels being more usua? rounded), The IPA diagram also supplies us with a number of additional symt for vowels in certain positions, [r,a,i,3,e] being used for unrounded vowels. [n,u,e] for rounded vowels. Notes 1 See Cmttenden (2013) for discussion of the use of these videos in teaching. 2 These are called post-alveolar on the chart of the International Phonetic Alph; (Table 1). 3 Go to http://ipa.group.shef.ac.uk to see and hear recordings of each sound in the In national Phonetic Alphabet. 4 See Duckworth et al. (1990). 5 Copies of the original recording of the Cardinal Vowels by Daniel Jones are on companion website. oUnds in language e | Speech sounds and linguistic units ■i r m have a way of classifying the sounds which can be produced by the speech t" ci'tan1- 'Pcech sound produced in isolation can be described in purely phonetic i -en l-ut any purely phonetic approach to the sounds of language encounters t . ti.ci'l" i because speech is normally a continuum of sound. Two initial problems ■( r.-ctn; first, the identification and delimitation of the sound unit (or segment) to be described and, second, the way in which different sounds are treated, for ■It purpose of linguistic analysis, as if they were the same. As we have seen, in any investigation of speech, it is on the physiological and - acoustic levels that most information is available to us. But in any articulation, as revealed by MRI, an utterance consists of apparently continuous movements by a very large number of organs; it is almost impossible to say, simply from a video of the speech organs at work, how many speech sounds have been uttered. A display of acoustic information is slightly easier to handle (see Fig. 3), but even here it is not always possible to delimit exactly the beginning and end of sound segments because of the way in which many sounds merge into one another. Moreover, even if we were able to delimit and identify certain sounds, it would not follow that all the individual units would fit into a useful linguistic description of the language being investigated. Thus, the word tot is frequently pronounced in the London region in such a way that it is possible to identify five sound segments: [t], [s], [h], [x>], [t]. Yet much of this phonetic reality may be discarded as irrelevant when it is a question of the structure of the word tot in terms of the sound system of English. Indeed, the speaker himself will probably : feel that the utterance tot consists of only three 'sounds', such a judgement on his pari being a highly sophisticated one which results from his experience in hearing and speaking English (and not only because of influence from the spelling). . In other words, the [s] and fh] segments are to be treated as part of the phonological, or linguistic, unit /t/.1 The phonetic sequence [tsh] does not, in an initial position in this type of English, consist of three meaningful units; in other lan-iguages, on the other hand, such a sequence might well constitute three linguistic units as well as three phonetic segments.