JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 12. - i ‘I-.w 1 ! 7 i i I The “False Consensus Effect”: An Egocentric Bias in Social Perception and Attribution Processes LEE Ross, DAVID GREENE, AND PAMELA HOUSE Stanford University Received April 21, 1976 Evidence from four studies demonstrates that social observers tend to perceive a “false consensus” with respect to the relative commonness oftheir own responses. A related bias was shown to exist in the observers’ social inferences. Thus, raters estimated particular responses to be relatively common and relatively unrevealing concerning the actors’ distinguishing personal dispositions when the responses in question were similar to the raters’ own responses; responses differing from those of the rater, by contrast, were perceived to be relatively uncommon and revealing of the actor. These results were obtained both in questionnaire studies presenting subjects with hypothetical situations and choices and in authentic conflict situations. The implications of these findings for our understanding of social perception phenomena and for our analysis of the divergent perceptions of actors and observers are discussed. Finally, cognitive and perceptual mechanisms are proposed which might account for distortions in perceived consensus and for corresponding biases in social inference and attributional processes. In a sense, every social observer is an intuitive psychologist who is forced by everyday experience to judge the causes and implications of behavior. Many researchers and theorists have expressed a general interest in naive epistemology and implicit psychological theories. However, it has been the attribution theorists (Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Jones, Kanouse, Kelley, Nisbett, Valins, & Weiner, 1972; Kelley, 1967, 1973) who have pursued this topic most vigorously and most systematically. The primary focus of formal attribution theory (following Kelley, 1967, 1973) has been the logical rules or “schemata” that the layman employs in making causal inferences and extracting social meaning from particular configurations of data. In the typical attribution study it has thus been the experimenter who has supplied the relevant data and, in so doing, has manipulated the degree of apparent response consistency, distinctiveness, and consensus presented to the social observer. This research strategy (e.g., McArthur, 1972) has obvious and undeniable advantages if the experimenter’s primary concern is the attributor’s rules for data analysis The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Michael Fleming who helped to design and execute Study 3and Study 4 and of Michael Hubbard who assisted in the analysis and interpretation of data from all four studies. David Greene is now at Carnegie-Mellon University. 279 Copyright 0 1977 by Academic Press. Inc. All rights of reproductmn in any form reserved. ISSN 0022.1031 280 ROSS. GREENE. AND HOUSE and data interpretation. But such a strategy also has serious costs, for it necessarily demands that one overlook those potentially crucial phases in the attribution process, preceding data analysis, during which the data must first be acquired, coded, and recalled from memory. The professional psychologist relies upon well-defined sampling techniques and statistical procedures for estimating the commonness of particular responses. Where such estimates are relevant to subsequent interpretations and inferences, he can proceed with confidence in his data. Intuitive psychologists, by contrast, are rarely blessed either with adequate “baseline” data or with the means of acquiring such data. To the extent that their systems for interpreting social responses depend upon estimates of commonness or oddity they must, accordingly, rely largely upon subjective impressions and intuitions. The source of attributional bias that we shall consider in the present paper directly involves the probability estimates made by intuitive psychologists. Specifically, we shall report research demonstrating that laymen tend to perceive a “false consensus”-to see their own behavioral choices and judgments as relatively common and appropriate to existing circumstances while viewing alternative responses as uncommon, deviant, or inappropriate. Evidence shall also be reported for an obvious corollary to the false consensus proposition: The intuitive psychologist judges those responses that differ from his own to be more revealing of the actor’s stable dispositions than those responses which are similar to his own. Thus, we contend that the person who feeds squirrels, votes Republican, or drinks Drambuie for breakfast will see such behaviors or choices by an actor as relatively common and relatively devoid of information about his personal characteristics. By contrast, another person who ignores hungry squirrels, votes Democrat, or abstains at breakfast will see the former actor’s responses as relatively odd and rich with implications about the actor’s personality. The term relative is critical in this formulation of the false consensus bias and it requires some clarification. Obviously, the man who would walk a tightrope between two skyscrapers, launch a revolution, or choose a life of clerical celibacy recognizes that his choices would be shared by few of his peers and are revealing of personal dispositions. It is contended, however, that he would see his personal choices as less deviant and revealing than would those of us who do not walk tightropes, launch revolutions, or become celibate clerics. Furthermore, the present thesis does not deny that “pluralistic ignorance” could lead to erroneous estimates by minority and majority alike. The incidence of infant abuse. for instance, might be underestimated by abusing and nonabusing parents alike. The relative terms of the false consensus thesis demand only that abusing parents estimate child abuse to be more common and less indicative of personal dispositions than do nonabusing parents. THE FALSE CONSENSUS EFFECT 281 References to “egocentric attribution” (Heider, 1958; Jones & Nisbett, 1972) to “attributive projection” (Holmes, 1968; Murstein & Pryer, 1959) and to a host of related projection phenomena (e.g., Cameron & Magaret, 1951; Cattell, 1944; Murray, 1933) have appeared sporadically in the literature. Perhaps most directly relevant to present concerns are empirical demonstrations of correlations between subjects’ own behavior and their estimates about their peers. For instance, Katz and Allport (1931) demonstrated that the admitted frequency of cheating by a student was positively related to his estimate of the number of other students who have cheated. Holmes (1968) summarized several other related demonstrations dealing with political beliefs and judgments of personal attributes and, more recently, Kelley and Stahelski (1970) have stressed the role of egocentric perceptions in the prisoner’s dilemma situation. In the present paper we shall demonstrate the generality of the false consensus or egocentric attribution bias. More importantly, we shall explore its implications for our understanding of social perception phenomena and the often divergent perceptions of actors and observers. Finally, we shall discuss more basic shortcomings of the intuitive psychologist which may underlie such phenomena. STUDY 1 Method Study 1 presented subjects with questionnaires containing one of four brief stories composed specifically for our purposes. A total of 320 Stanford undergraduates (80 for each of the four stories) participated. Each story asked the readers to place themselves in aparticular setting in which a series of events culminated in a clear behavioral choice. The subjects were not immediately required to state their own choice but were asked to estimate the percentage of their peers who would choose each ofthe two possible courses ofaction suggested. The four stories and the consensus questions are reproduced below: SUPERMARKET STORY As you are leaving your neighborhood supermarket a man in a business suit asks you whether you like shopping in that store. You reply quite honestly that you do like shopping there and indicate that in addition to being close to your home the supermarket seems to have very good meats and produce at reasonably low prices. The man then reveals that avideotape crew has filmed your comments and asks you to sign arelease allowing them to use the unedited film for aTV commercial that the supermarket chain is preparing. What % of your peers do you estimate would sign the release?-% What % would refuse to sign it?-% (Total % should be 100%) TERM PAPER STORY You arrive for the first day of class in a course in your major area of study. The professor saysthat the grade in your course will depend on apaper due the final day of the course. He gives the class the option of two alternatives upon which they 282 ROSS, GREENE. AND HOUSE must vote. They can either do papers individually in the normal way or they can work in teams of three persons who will submit a single paper between them. Your are informed that he will still give out the same number of A’s, B’s, and C’s, etc., but that in the first case every student will be graded individually while the second case all three students who work together get the same grade. What % of your peers do you estimate would vote for group papers?-% What % would vote for individual papers?-% (Total % should be 100%) TRAFFIC TICKET STORY While driving through a rural area near your home you are stopped by a county police officer who informs you that you have been clocked (with radar) at 38 miles per hour in a 25-mph zone. You believe this information to be accurate. After the policeman leaves, you inspect your citation and find that the details on the summons regarding weather, visibility, time, and location of violation are highly inaccurate. The citation informs you that you may either pay a $20 fine by mail without appearing in court or you must appear in municipal court within the next two weeks to contest the charge. What % of your peers do you estimate would pay the $20 fine by mail?-% What % would go to court to contest the charge?-% (Total should be 100%) SPACE PROGRAM REFERENDUM STORY It is proposed in Congress that the space program be revived and that large sums be allocated for the manned and unmanned exploration of the moon and planets nearest Earth. Supporters of the proposal argue that it will provide jobs. spur technology, and promote national pride and unity. Opponents argue that a space program will either necessitate higher taxes, or else dram money from important domestic priorities. Furthermore, they deny that it will accomplish the desirable effects claimed by the program’s supporters. Both sides, of course, refute each other’s claims and ultimately a public referendum is held. What % of your peers do you estimate would vote for the proposed allocation of funds for space exploration?-% What % would vote against it?-% (Total should be 100%) After completing the relevant percentage estimates, subjects were required to fill out a three-page questionnaire. On one page, they were first asked to indicate which of the two behavioral options they personally would choose and then asked to rate themselves on a series of Likert-type personality scales. This self description page of the questionnaire was either followed or preceded by two pages on which the reader was required to rate the persona1 traits of the “typical person” of the reader’s own age and sex who would choose each of the two specific options presented in the story. For example, subjects who read the Supermarket Story were required on one page to rate the traits of “the typical person . who would sign the commercial release” and, on another page. to rate “the typical person . who would not sign the commercial release. ” The order of these three rating sheets was systematically varied.’ I In the interests of brevity and clarity, neither self-ratings nor the “order of presentation” variable receives detailed consideration in this report, It should be noted only that self-ratings were irrelevant to our present hypotheses and, similarly, that the order variable produced no significant main effects or interaction effects of immediate theoretical relevance. THE FALSE CONSENSUS EFFECT 283 The nature of the rating scales merits some emphasis. For each story, the actors were rated with respect to a different set of four personal characteristics that might influence or be reflected by the behavioral choice described in the story. (In the Supermarket Story, for example, the traits considered were shyness, cooperativeness, trust, and adventurousness.) For each trait a IOO-point rating scale was used. The midpoint and two extremes of this scale were labeled to specify both the extremity of the dispositional inference made and the rater’s conjdence concerning the relevant actors’ dispositions. Thus, for the disposition of “cooperativeness”, the scale was anchored as follows: +50 Certainly more cooperative than the average person of my age and sex,probably very much more cooperative. 0 Probably average with respect to cooperativeness. I have no reason to assume that the actor differs from the average person of my age and sex with respect to this characteristic. -50 Certainly less cooperative than the average person of my age and sex,probably very much less cooperative. Summary ofhypotheses. The consensus estimates and trait ratings for each story provided a test of two principal hypotheses: I. Subjects who “choose” a particular hypothetical response will rate that response as more probable for “people in general” than will subjects who “choose” the alternative response. 2. Subjects who “choose” a specified response will use less extreme and less confident trait ratings in characterizing a “typical” person making that response than will subject who “choose” the alternative response. TABLE 1 PERCEIVED CONSENSUS: ESTIMATED COMMONNESS OF OWN AND ALTERNATIVE BEHAVIORAL CHOICES (STUDY I) Rater’s own choice in hypothetically Story described situation n (%) Supermarket Sign release 53 (66%) story Not sign release 27 (34%) Term paper Choose individual 64 (80%) story paper Choose papergroup 16 (20%) Traffic ticket Pay speeding fine 37 (46%) story Contest charge 43 (54%) Space program Vote for cutback 32 (40%) story Vote against cutback 48(60%) Summary of Choose option 1 186 (58%) four stories” Choose option 2 I34 (42%) a Unweighted average of means for four stories. Estimates of consensus: estimated percentage of raters who would choose Option 1 Option 2 F 75.6 24.4 17.7 57.3 42.7 67.4 32.6 16.5 45.9 54.1 71.8 28.2 12.8 51.7 48.3 47.9 52.1 4.9 39.0 61.0 65.7 34.3 49. I 48.5 51.5 284 ROSS, GREENE, AND HOUSE Results Perceptions of consensus. The data presented in Table 1 offer strong support for the first of the experimental hypotheses. For each of the stories, those subjects who claimed that they personally would follow a given behavioral alternative also tended to rate that alternative as relatively probable for “people in general”; those subjects who claimed that they would reject the alternative tended to rate it as relatively improbable for “people in general.” The effect of subjects’ own behavior choice upon their estimates of commonness was statistically significant for each story individually. When Story was treated as a “fixed” variable in an analysis of variance combining the data for all four stories, the main effect of Rater’s Choice was highly significant, F( 1,312) = 49.1, p < .OOl, while the Story x Rater’s Choice interaction was trivial, F( 1,312) = 1.37, p > .lO. It is clear that the Rater’s reported behavioral choices were associated with, and presumably exerted a large and consistent effect upon, their perceptions of behavioral consensus. Trait ratings. To test the second experimental hypothesis individual trait TABLE 3 TRAIT RATINGS: RATERS’ INFERENCES ABOUT PERSONAL DISPOSITIONS OF “TYPICAL ACTORS” BEHAVING LIKE OR UNLIKE THE RATER (STUDY 1) S10ry Supermarket story Term paper story Trafhc ticket Sf”,Y space program S,“,Y Summary of four storwsh TraIta Shyness C”“peratl”eness TrUst Adventurousness Gregariousness LEIZl”ess Competitweneaa G~~~r”Slly Self-coniidence Legal knowledge Argumenfativemw Miserlmess Rationality Social concern Idealism Selfishness Rater‘\ own choice sgn release ,n = Si) Not sag” release In = 27) Choose mdivldual grade ,n = 641 Choose group grade cn = 161 Pay speedmg hne ,n = 371 Contest charge cn = 431 Vote for cutback In = 32, Vote agamst cutback IfI = 48) Choose opt,“” I Choose option ? Trait ratings:” Rater‘s asseaament of person who would choose DlffW OptIon I OptIon 2 ence t 60.4 x9 5 -29.1 17.27 90.0 65 ? +?‘I.7 66.4 66.6 -0 2 21.83 loo.9 46.8 +54.1 35 8 77.4 -41.6 3 IX 59.4 79.? - 19.2 56 X 67 9 -III I.XI 6X.‘) 6X.9 0 54.9 7S.J -20 5 37.40 79.x 65.1 +14.7 a Larger numbers reflect stronger trait inferencrr and greater willmgness t” assert that the person would prox denant or discrepant from average with respect to the four .pecltied tranz b Unweighted average of means for Sour ‘tone>. THE FALSE CONSENSUS EFFECT 285 ratings were measured as absolute discrepancies from the midpoint of the loo-point Likert-type scale. Ratings for the four traits for each story were then combined to provide an overall measure of the rater’s inferences about the “typical” actor who might choose each of the behavioral alternatives specified. The results are summarized in Table 2. The pattern of trait ratings for each story is in accord with the research hypothesis. That is, relatively strong inferences about the typical person who might choose a given response are made by those raters who personally would choose the alternative response. The difference scores in Table 2 are the critical indices; in each case this difference score is more positive (or less negative) for the rater who would choose the second alternative over the first. For two of the stories (Supermarket Story and Term Paper Story), the effect of Rater’s Own Choice upon the difference in ratings for the two behavioral alternatives was clearly significant, F(1,78) = 17.27,~ < .OOl andF = 21.83,~ < .OOl, respectively. Forone story (Traffic Ticket Story) the effect was marginally significant, F = 3.18, p < .lO, and for one story (Space Program Story) the effect was relatively trivial, F = 1.81,~ < .lO. The four stories again may be considered within a single analysis of variance. When Story is treated as a fixed variable, the main effect of the subject’s own behavioral choice upon the difference in trait ratings is highly significant, F(l,312) = 37.40, p < .OOl. The Story x Subject’s Choice Interaction, however, is also significant, F(3,312) = 3.71,~ < .05.2 Thus, like perceptions of behavioral consensus, trait inferences are systematically influenced by the rater’s own behavioral choices, although the effect is less consistent for the latter measure than the former. An obvious question arises concerning the relationship between the two effects that have been demonstrated. However, consideration of this question, and other questions concerning alternative interpretations and underlying mechanisms, shall be postponed until the procedures and principal results for the remaining three studies have been reported. STUDY 2 Rationale and Method Study 2 attempted to extend the domain of the false consensus effect. Whereas Study 1had demonstrated that subjects tend to overestimate the degree of consensus enjoyed by their own behavioral choices in ahypothetical conflict situation, Study 2was designed to explore a more 2When Story is treated as a random variable (so that the “Mean Square” for Story x Rater’s Choice interaction constitutes the appropriate error term) the F ratio for the main effect of Rater’s Choice is reduced to 10.1: this F ratio (with only 3 degrees of freedom associated with its lesser mean square) is only marginally significant (p = .05). A more discriminating and appropriate test of the generality of the hypothesis, of course, would require a far Larger sample of stories than that employed in Study I. 286 ROSS, GREENE, AND HOUSE general tendency for subjects to overestimate the extent to which others share their habits, preferences, fears, daily activities, expectations, and other personal characteristics.3 The procedure was simple. A total of 80 Stanford undergraduates completed a questionnaire dealing with 35 person description items (see Table 3). Each item presented a pair of mutually exclusive and exhaustive categories. Half of the subjects first categorized themselves with respect to the 35 variables and then proceeded to estimate the percentage of “college students in general” who fit into each category: the remainder answered these questions in reverse order. (The “order variable” produced no relevant main effects or interaction effects and, accordingly, receives no further consideration in this report.) The hypothesis in Study 2 was simply that subjects who placed themselves in a given personal description category would estimate the percentage of “college students in general” in that category to be greater than would subjects who placed themselves in the alternative category. Results The personal description items (listed in the same order presented to subjects) and the relevant percentage estimates made by subjects are summarized in Table 3. One of the 3.5items, a Political Expectation item concerning the impeachment of President Nixon, became unusable in the midst of the study. Each of the remaining 34 items separately tested the false consensus hypothesis (although the tests were not “independent” since the same group of subjects responded to all items). A quick inspection of the results presented in the table reveals considerable, although less than universal, support for the hypothesis. Overall, the difference in percentage estimates was in the predicted direction for 32 of the 34 items, and the magnitude of the two reversals was trivial. That is, subjects who placed themselves in a given descriptive category consistently estimated the percentage of “college students in general” in that category to be greater than did subjects who placed themselves in the alternative category. Of the items showing the predicted effect, 17 produced differences significant beyond the .10 level while 11 differences were significant beyond the .01 level. Further examination of the data reveals that three of the seven categories provided fairly strong and consistent support for the false consensus hypothesis. Most dramatic were the items pertaining to Political Expectations: Subjects who expected women soon to be appointed to the Supreme Court, poverty to abate, nuclear weapons to be used, or extraterrestrial life to be discovered, perceived these views to be relatively widespread among their peers; subjects with the opposite expectations similarly thought that their own expectations were characteristic of “college students in general.” Less dramatic but reasonably consistent support was also provided by these items dealing with Personal Traits and Views and those probing Personal Problems. For example, subjects who 3The full text of this questionnaire is too lengthy to be reproduced in the present report, although Table 3 does list all of the relevant personal description categories. The questionnaire itself may be obtained from the first author. THE FALSE CONSENSUS EFFECT 287 TABLE 3 RATERS' SELF-CATEGORIZATIONS ANDTHEIRCATEGORIZATIONSOF "COLLEGE STUDENTS IN GENERAL"(STUDY 2) Questionnaire item: category 1 (category 2) Raters‘ estimates of percentage of college students in category I DIrection of difference Mean estimates by Mean estmates by (+ predicted: raters placmg them- raters placing them- opporite selver in category I selves in category 2 to predicted) I Personal traits and views Shy (not shy) Optimistic (not) Competitive (not) Politically left of center (not) Supporter of women’s lib (not) Unweighted mean of live items 45.9 35.9 + 61.9 50.4 + 75.1 6Y.9 + 59.7 5X.0 + 57.3 33.4 + 60.0 49.5 Personal preferences Brown (white) bread To be alone (with others) Italian (French) movies City (country) life Basketball (football) Unwaghted mean of five items 52.5 36.0 51.6 51.4 36.7 45.6 Personal characteristics Male (female) Brown (blue) eyes Subscribe (don’t) to magazines on list provided First-born (laterborn) child Hometown more (less) than 200,000 Unweighted mean of five items 5x.7 57. I + 58.3 54.5 + 56.9 42.7 + 42.2 37. I + 5x.2 51.9 + 54.9 4x.7 Personal problems Think about dying? yes (no) 44.0 Hard to make friends? yes (no) 3x.7 Difficulty controlling temper? yes (no) 42.1 Frequently depressed? yes (no) 55. I Emotional needs satisfied? yes (no) 52.9 Unwaghted mean of tive items 46.6 Personal activities Watch TV 30 hours/month? yes (no) Play tennis once a week? yes (no) Attend religious service once a month? yes (no) Donate blood once a year? yes (no) Long distance phone call once a week? yes (no) Unwelghted means of tive wms 49.2 33.0 26.5 22.6 50.7 36.4 Personal expectations Marriage by age 30? yes (no) Better financial status than parent? yes (no) Live outside U.S. for one year in next 20? yes (II”) 74.5 6x.3 Great satisfaction from job or career? yes (no) Death before 70th birthday? yes (no) Unwetghted mean of five items 37.4 53.5 57.6 58.3 Political expectations Removal of Nixon from office? yes (no) Woman in Supreme Court within decade? yes (noI Deleted 63.3 34.6 + 6.196 37.4 + 30.7 + 43.4 + 49.8 + 37.7 39.x 25.6 + 35. I + 27.9 + 39.2 + 42.2 + 34.0 2.664 2.w 1.35