78 THEORIES OF ETHNICITY FREDRIK BARTH 79 cultural tradition,. but which makes it possible to separate these factors and investigate the non-ecologicalcultural and social components creating diver- .sity. The cultural features that signal the ~oundary may change~ and th~ cultural characreristics of the members may likewise be transformed, indeed, even the organízational form of the group may change-vyet the fact of continuing dichotomization between members and outsiders allows us to specify the nature of continuity, and investigate the changing cultural form and content. 2. Sodally relevant factors alone become diagnostic for membership, not the overt, 'objective' differences which are generated by other facrors. It makes no difference how dissimilar members may be in their overt behaviour-if they say they are A, in contrast to another cognate category B, they are willing to be treated and let their own behaviour be interpreted and judged as Ks and not as B's; in other words, they declare their allegiance to the shared culture of A's. The effects of this, as compared to other factors influendng actual behaviour, can then be made the object of investigation. Ethnic groups as an organizational type Byconcentrating on what is socially effective, ethnic groups are seen as a form of social organization. The critical feature then becomes item (4) in the list above, the characteristic of self-ascription and ascription by others, A categorical ascription is an ethnic ascription when it classifies a person in terms of his basic, most general identity, presumptively determined by his origin and background. To the extent that actors use ethnic identities to categorize themselves and others for purposes of interaction, they form erhnic groups in this organizational sense. It is important to recognize that although ethnic categories take cultural differences into account, we can assume no simple one-to-one relationship between ethnic units and cultural similarities and differences. The fearures that are taken into account are not the sum ofobjective' differences, but only those which the actors themselves regard as significant. Not only do ecologic variations mark and exaggerate differences; some cultural features are used by the actors as signals and emblems of differences, others are ignored, and in some relationships radical differences are played down and denied. The cultural contents of ethnic dichotomies would seem analytical1y to be of two orders: (i) overr signals or signs-the diacritical fearures that people look for and exhibit to show identity, often such features as dress, language, houseform, or general style oflife, and (ii) basic value orientations: the standards of morality and excellence by which performance is judged. Since belonging to an ethnic category implies being a certain kind of person, having that basic identity, it also implies a claim to be judged, and to judge oneself, by those standards that are relevant to that identity. Neither of these kinds of cultural 'contents' follows from a descriptive list of cultural fearures or cultural differences; one cannot predict from first principles which features will be emphasized and made organizationally relevant by the actors. In other words, ethnic categories provide an organizational vessel that may be given varying arnounts and forms of content in different socio-culrural systems. They may be of great relevance to behaviour, but they need not be; they may pervade all sociallife, or they may be relevant only in límited sectors of activity. There is thus an obvious scope for ethnographic and comparative descriptions of different forms of ethnic organization. The emphasis on ascription as the critical feature of ethnic groups also solves the two conceptual difficulties that were discussed above. I. When defined as an ascriptive and exclusive group, the nature of centinuity of ethnic units is clear: it depends on the maintenance of a boundary. The boundaries oj ethnic groups The critical focus of investigation from this point of view becomes the ethnic boundary that defines the gr~up, not the cultural stuff that it encloses. The boundaries to which we rnust give our attention are of course soda I boundarie, though they may have territorial counterparts. If a group maintains its identity when members interact with others, this entails criteria for dererrnining membership and ways of signalling membership and exclusion. Ethnic groups are merely or necessarily based on the occupation of exclusive territories; and the different ways in which they are maintained, not only by a once-and-for-all recruitment but by continual expression and validation, need to be analysed. What is more, the ethnic boundary canalizes social \ife-it entails a frequently quite complex organization of behaviour and social relations. The identification of another person as a fellow member of an ethnic group imp\ies a sharing of criteria for evaluation and judgement. It thus entails the assumption that the twť are fundamentally 'playing the same garne', and this means that there is berween them a potential for diversification and expansion of their socíal relationship to cover eventually all different sectors and domains of activity. On the other hand, a dichotomization of others as strangers, as members of another ethnic group, implies a recognition of limitations on shared understandings, differences in criteria for judgement of value and performance, and a restriction of interaction to sectors of assumed common understanding and mutual interest. This makes it possible to understand one final form of boundary maintenahce whereby cultural units and boundaries persist. Entailed in ethnic boundary maintenance are also siruarions of soda 1contact between persons of dífferenr cultures: ethnic groups only persist as significant units if they imply marked difference in behaviour, i.e. persisting cultural differences. Yet