Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 brill.com/nu © koninklijke brill nv, leiden, ���4 | doi 10.1163/15685276-12341319 Long-lost Brothers: On the Co-histories and Interactions Between the Comparative Science of Religion and the Anthropology of Religion1 Armin W. Geertz Aarhus University, Department of Culture and Society, Section for the Study of Religion, Religion, Cognition and Culture Research Unit (rcc), Jens Chr. Skous Vej 3, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark awg@teo.au.dk Abstract This article briefly surveys and compares the histories of research in the comparative science of religion (beginning with Friedrich Max Müller) and the anthropology of religion. The article notes the close interactions between these two fields and argues that the comparative science of religion drew significant inspiration from anthropology and sociology during the twentieth century until about the 1970s when anthropology came under heavy fire from critics. The postcolonial, feminist, and postmodern wave did not have a significant impact on the comparative science of religion until the 1990s. But already during the 1980s a new approach to religion, championed by Jonathan Z. Smith, contributed to a theoretical and critical analysis of religion that neither bought into postmodernism nor into the sui generis approach to religion. During the 1990s, another new approach began making an impact, namely, the cognitive science of religion, championed by E.Thomas Lawson, Robert N. McCauley (both scholars of religion), and Pascal Boyer (anthropologist). The article suggests in conclusion that the two disciplines can once again meet in the growing fields of experimental anthropology and experimental science of religion and in the need to explore and address how culture affects and rewires the brain. Furthermore, evolutionary theory is also beginning to serve as a common framework for thinking about religion. 1 This review essay was written with an anthropological audience in mind, but I hope that my argument will also be of interest to scholars of religion because it attempts to point a way forward for the scientific and analytical study of religion. 256 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Keywords history of religions – comparative science of religion – anthropology of religion – cognitive science of religion – evolution – method – theory – psychology – sociology – history The Big-brother-little-brother Histories of Our Fields The comparative science of religion was programmatically introduced by the German expatriate at Oxford, Friedrich Max Müller during the middle of the nineteenth century. His ambitious goal was to develop the comparative sciences of linguistics, mythology, religion, and thought. In many ways, his ambition is reflected by trends in the study of religion today, to which I will return. Müller was influenced by the times, of course, where the new comparativism was shaping all the sciences and changing the very fabric of European and American conceptions of the world.2 He began a field that came to be known as Religionswissenschaft. It consisted of comparative histories of religions, translations, and interpretations of religious texts, and the development of theories of religious evolution and methodological debate. It was mostly promoted by orientalists and classicists. Besides Müller himself, names like Pierre D. Chantepie de la Saussaye, William Robertsen Smith, Andrew Lang, and Sir James George Frazer come to mind. A signal characteristic of the comparative science of religion is its methodological pluralism. There is no single method in that science.The methods employed by scholars of religion have been developed elsewhere: history, linguistics, archaeology, philosophy, and so on. Towards the latter half of the nineteenth century, experimental psychology, anthropology, and sociology arose as new disciplines. It was an exciting time, fevered by evolutionary theory and cross-fertilizations between disciplines. Everyone was interested in religion and culture within an evolutionary framework. Out of the closing decades of that century strong disciplinary paradigms emerged into the early twentieth century, associated with such names asWilliam James and Sigmund Freud in psychology, Edward B.Tylor and Franz Boas in anthropology, and Émile Durkheim and Max Weber in sociology. 2 See Geertz, McCutcheon, and Elliot 2000; Geertz and McCutcheon 2000. For overviews of the history of the comparative science of religion or Religionswissenschaft — “religionsvidenskab” in Danish — see especially the loose trilogy of studies of the history of research: Waardenburg 1973–1974; Whaling 1983–1985; and Antes, Geertz, and Warne 2004. 257Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 As the evolutionary paradigm faded, the functionalist movement took over with Bronislaw Malinowski as its main proponent. In the U. S., however, during the first decades of the twentieth century, anthropology was heavily influenced by psychology and the culture-and-personality approach in the work of such scholars as Robert H. Lowie, Paul Radin, Ruth Benedict, and also psychoanalysts like Georges Devereux and Géza Róheim. The comparative science of religion came to be dominated by psychologists and theologians with religious agendas such as Rudolf Otto, Carl G. Jung, Friedrich Heiler, Wilfred Cantwell Smith, Joachim Wach, Mircea Eliade, and others.3 Historians of religions retreated to an ideographic, historicist methodological paradigm, eschewing broad generalizations and especially what were perceived of as metaphysical speculations. The comparative discipline of the study of religion became the “phenomenology of religion” and was divided into two strands: the strand heavily influenced by philosophical phenomenology, i.e., Gerardus van der Leeuw, and the strand that was historical-­typological, i.e., Raffaele Pettazzoni and Ugo Bianchi. The results were theoretically uninformed concepts and terms without any general framework. It was an ad hoc endeavor, “the mere multiplication of generalized cover terms,” as Clifford Geertz rightly observed in his criticism of Mircea Eliade (1968:23). For all intents and purposes, scholars of religion looked up to anthropology and sociology, especially the former, as the big brothers of the comparative study of religion because throughout most of the twentieth century, all the main theories of religion came from those within these disciplines, such as Talcott Parsons, S. N. Eisenstadt, Edward Shils, Michael Kearney, Claude Lévi-Strauss, C. Geertz, Victor Turner, Peter Berger, Robert Bellah, Meredith B. McGuire, and Danièle Hervieu-Léger, just to name a few. With few exceptions, historians of religions were not producing interesting or useful generalizations or theories.4 3 Even today psychologists of religion are struggling with these tendencies. The well-known empirical psychologist of religion, David M. Wulff, has recently argued that they need to start all over again (2003). 4 A singular exception is Walter Burkert who quite early on was concerned with the origins of religion in hunting rituals (1972, 1996). Philosopher René Girard introduced his mimesis hypothesis at the same time (1972), and these two scholars had an impact on the study of religion (Hamerton-Kelly 1988). Another two exceptions are the Belgian classicist Marcel Detienne (1981; Detienne and Vernant 1974) and French classicist Jean-Pierre Vernant (1962, 1965) who applied structural theory to Greek texts. 258 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 When Big Brother Lost His Nerve During the 1970s, however, things began to change. Anthropology still had its strong fieldwork identity, but it began losing its nerve under the assaults of feminists, philosophical hermeneutists, representatives of former colonies, and postmodern movements. Suddenly anthropology and anthropological fieldwork were reduced to mainly Western male self-representations expressing and generalizing the tropes of colonial racism and suppression. Cultural relativism, always implicit in anthropology, took over. The results were mixed: necessary foundational reflection and debate on the one hand, but rampant ethnic, genderized, ideological, and religious power struggles on the other. As C. Geertz noted, the study of human cultures are indeed constructions. What else could they be? This fact does not detract from their claims of actuality or truth (Geertz 1995:62). The comparative study of religion was not affected by those movements until the 1990s when they became firmly entrenched in the programs of the American Academy of Religion (aar).The aar is the largest professional association of Religious Studies in the United States. It originally grew out of the National Association of Biblical Instructors (nabi). Their well-known critic, Toronto-based scholar and philosopher of religion, Donald Wiebe, argued that the aar continued the religio-theological agenda of the old nabi (1999:238). By this he did not mean ecclesiastical interference in the study of religion, rather he meant the assumption that the study of religion in the university is a sui generis enterprise, dictated by the special status of religion (i.e., its transcendent element) in a Christian sense. Into the 1960s, religious and theological pluralism changed the narrow Christian focus to a more ecumenical one, but the real tension was between practicing and celebrating religion, on the one hand, and critically studying it, on the other. A recent Executive Director of the aar stated specifically that it was no longer possible to assume “a clear distinction separating scholarship from advocacy” (Frisinia 1993:5; quoted in Wiebe 1999:249). The ecumenical idea of “conversation” replaced analysis, interpretation, and explanation, thus allowing fertile ground for various religious and ideological agendas. Things have been changing, however, during the past decade or so, where new approaches and analytical groups have been manifesting themselves within the framework of the American Academy of Religion and in the Journal of the American Academy of Religion.5 The fact that the aar has become a 5 Recent President of the aar, Ann Taves (2010, 2011), is an excellent example of the new turn in that scholarly organization. 259Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 member of the International Association for the History of Religions (iahr), the only international professional organization of the comparative study of religion, is a further indication of promising developments. In terms of the post-colonial critique, things are also changing, or perhaps they have always been like this, and we “Westerners” simply had not noticed it in the midst of all our existential agony. The postcolonial scholars I know in African and Native American studies are mostly just asking scholars, regardless of their ethnic or national backgrounds, to show respect for the people and the cultures they study, to observe ethical standards, and to do their work with the utmost scientific care and responsibility. In other words: do your job properly.6 How can we disagree with that? As C. Geertz argued in his entertaining essay “Anti Anti-Relativism,” in choosing between the relativist’s worry about our provincialism and the anti-relativist’s worry about everything becoming relative and meaningless, he clearly finds “provincialism altogether the more real concern so far as what actually goes on in the world” (Geertz 1984:46). When Little Brother Grew Up The postmodern tendency in American religious studies was offset by what we call the analytical turn in the study of religion, begun by Jonathan Z. Smith, professor of the history of religions in Chicago. His agenda was explicitly against the sui generis approach to religion, championed, among others, by his predecessor Mircea Eliade. The title of his seminal essay collection, Map Is Not Territory (1978), basically encapsulates his argument. Religion, he claimed, is primarily an analytical construction used to interpret certain kinds of human behavior, and it is imperative that we distinguish between construct and empirical behavior.7 The analytical turn in the study of religion led to the 6 Leading names here are of course Talal Asad, David Chidester, Gerrie ter Haar, Rosalind I. J. Hackett, Tomoko Masuzawa, Jacob K. Olupona, Jan G. Platvoet, Abdulkader I. Tayob, Jace Weaver, and many others. See my contributions to the postcolonial debate and my attempts to formulate an ethnohermeneutic alternative (A. Geertz 1994a, 1994b, 2003, and 2004a). 7 Leading scholars of the analytical turn in North America were Gregory D. Alles, William Arnal, Gustavo Benavides, Willi Braun, Sam Gill, Abrahim H. Khan, E. Thomas Lawson, Gary Lease, Bruce Lincoln, Luther H. Martin, Russell McCutcheon, William E. Paden, Hans H. Penner, Bryan Rennie, Benson Saler, Robert A. Segal, Ivan Strenski, and Donald Wiebe. Note that these are all men. This does not mean that women scholars were not also a part of the analytical turn. The main tradition in the analytical turn, however, was feminism, which was concerned with gender mainly inspired by French and German philosophy and various theological thinkers and in this sense was a different kind of analysis. Those feminists who­ 260 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 establishment of a professional association, the North American Association for the Study of Religion (naasr), which became affiliated to the iahr. naasr also founded a journal called Method and Theory in the Study of Religion that specifically called for research on the methods, theories, concepts, and themes of the academic, comparative science of religion. The results are an array of articles and books reflecting a naturalistic, critical, and comparative study (some maintain the term “science”) of religion. This more theoretically oriented movement resonated well with Scandinavian scholars because there already was a strong tradition, especially in Finland, of methodological reflection. Lauri Honko is the leading name in this respect because of his interdisciplinary approach combining folkloristics and fieldwork with the history of religions. He and his students have consistently discussed and published on methodology.8 In Sweden, Åke Hultkrantz was also a leading spokesman of methodological reflection. Again, we witness a historian of religions who conducted anthropological fieldwork and necessarily assumed a methodological pluralism.9 During recent decades, scholars in Norway have also contributed significantly to method and theory debates, such as the work of Ingvild Gilhus, Einar Thomassen, and Michael Stausberg.10 In Denmark, scholars in Aarhus have participated actively in the analytical turn.11 This turn had a strong influence on interdisciplinary debate and research cooperation at the Department of the Study of Religion for several decades.Through a series of conferences, anthologies, and special issues of the Aarhus journal Religionsvidenskabeligt Tidsskrift, colleagues have systematically explored methodological, theoretical, and conceptual themes in relation identified themselves with the analytical study of religion are, among others, Rosalind I. J. Hackett, Marsha Hewitt, Morny Joy, Ursula King, and Sylvia Marcos. 8 Honko’s edited volume Science of Religion: Studies in Methodology (1979) is an excellent example of this interest as well as his methodological work in folkloristics, such as Tradition and Cultural Identity (1988). Outstanding theoretically oriented Finnish scholars since Honko are Veikko Anttonen (Pyysiäinen and Anttonen 2002), Matti Kamppinen (1989), and Ilkka Pyysiäinen (2001). 9 His book in Swedish on methodology (1973) has been used in many university courses in the Nordic countries. 10 See various contributions in Kraft and Natvig 2006; Gilhus 2011; Thomassen 2007; and especially Stausberg (2009; Stausberg and Engler 2011; Kreinath, Snoek, and Stausberg 2006). There is also a growing interdisciplinary environment in Norway, seen for instance the anthology Norges religionshistorie (Amundsen 2005). 11 See the overview in Danish by Geertz and Jensen 2003. 261Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 to the philosophy of science.12 The work of Jeppe Sinding Jensen on conceptual and theoretical problems in the comparative science of religion has been particularly useful in this respect.13 In Copenhagen and Odense, scholars have excelled in ideographic studies of particular religions, but more theoretically reflective publications have also been produced.14 A younger generation has specifically identified itself with the analytical turn, but very few have found employment in the research community.15 My focus is on Scandinavia, but one could easily mention theoretically oriented scholars in Britain,16 the Netherlands,17 Germany,18 and other European countries.19 It is this general movement that most scholars outside of the field of comparative religion are unaware of. But it is imperative that they look into it — anthropologists, psychologists, historians, archaeologists, and cognitivists — if they wish to know what is at the forefront in comparative religion.20 12 For instance, the problem of definitions of religion (Bilde 1991 and Geertz 1999), comparison in the study of religion (Albinus 2005; Jensen 1993; Jensen 1999; Jensen 2000; Petersen 2011; Petersen 2013; Schjødt 2008), the problem of rationality (Albinus 2010; Jensen and Martin 1997), and so on. 13 His book The Study of Religion in a New Key (2003) is a major contribution to the theoretical and philosophical aspects of the comparative study of religion. 14 See Olav Hammer (Hammer and Lewis 2007; Hammer and von Stuckrad 2007), Tim Jensen and Mikael Rothstein (2000), Erik Reenberg Sand and Jørgen Podemann Sørensen (1999), Tove Tybjerg (2010), and Margit Warburg (2006). 15 See Torben Hammersholt (2011; Hammersholt and Schaffalitzky de Muckadell 2011), Anders Lisdorf and Joel Haviv (Haviv et al. 2005), and Oluf Schönbeck (Buck et al. 1997), just to mention a few. 16 Such as Brian Bocking, James L. Cox, Steven Engler, Graham Harvey, Kim Knott, Michael Pye, Steven Sutcliffe, and Alan V. Williams. 17 Such as Gerrie ter Haar, Wouter J. Hanegraff, Willem Hofstee, Jan G. Platvoet, and Kocku von Stuckrad. 18 Such as Wanda Alberts, Peter Antes, Ulrich Berner, Hans Kippenberg, Christoph Kleine, and Katja Triplett. 19 Such as Giovanni Casadio, Aleš Chalupa, Mihály Hoppál, and Panayotis Pachis. 20 Strangely, many scholars who are interested in religion, but not trained in comparative religion, often begin with William James, then jump to Mircea Eliade, and from there to Pascal Boyer and Daniel Dennett! 262 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Will the Long-lost Brothers Meet Again? Out of the conflict between postmodern religious studies and analytical, scientific studies appeared a completely new approach in 1990, dubbed the cognitive science of religion (csr). It hit not only the comparative religion field, but also anthropology. And both disciplines are still punch-drunk from the collision. Suddenly there were colleagues going around claiming that they could explain religion through the use of scientific methodologies.21 The initial reaction to the cognitive science of religion among comparative religion scholars was a cautious skepticism. This is understandable because of the general skepticism towards the psychology of religion. Anthropologists, however, have a long tradition of studying human societies, cultures, and psyches, more specifically in the subfield of psychological anthropology. Anthropologists have also developed cognitive approaches long before the cognitive science of religion appeared on the scene in 1990. Stephen Tyler, Dan Sperber, Stewart Guthrie, Benson Saler, Dorothy Holland, and Naomi Quinn all come to mind here. It was and still is, however, a small subfield.22 In all fairness, it seems that many anthropologists (and scholars of religion) were reacting against “the-mind-as-computer-cognitivists” who rejected any cultural influence on cognition. I am critical of that approach too. In fact, the Religion,CognitionandCultureresearchunit(rcc)thatIheadexplicitlyoffers an alternative to the so-called crum model (Computational-Representational Understanding of Mind), humorously labeled by philosopher Paul Thagard.23 21 The title of one of the cognitive anthropologist Pascal Boyer’s books is symptomatic: Religion Explained (2001). His assumptions were shared by pioneers E. Thomas Lawson and Robert N. McCauley (1990) and Harvey Whitehouse (1995), cf. my introduction to the field in A. Geertz 2004b. 22 See Tyler 1969; Sperber 1974; Sperber 1982; Sperber and Wilson 1986; Guthrie 1980; Saler 2009 (collected earlier essays); Holland and Quinn 1987. Michael Lambek’s recent reader in the anthropology of religion is apparently indicative of the majority of anthropologists: there is no mention of cognitive anthropology or the cognitive science of religion (2002). It should be noted that the Finnish scholar of religion, Matti Kamppinen, was one of the earliest in Scandinavia to publish on cognition (see his 1986, 1988, 1989, and 1993). 23 Thagaard 1996:10. See also his delightful book 2010. Note a few selected rcc publications: A. Geertz 2004b; A. Geertz 2010; Geertz and Jensen 2011; Jensen 2002; Jensen 2010; Martin and Sørensen 2011; and Sørensen 2007a. See also rcc confederate Ann Taves (1999, 2009). 263Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 We argue that cognition is embodied and embrained,24 situated,25 extended,26 distributed,27 materialized,28 and deeply cultural.29 Promising Avenues? Some promising avenues where the cognitive science of religion and the anthropology of religion can meet are in the growing fields of experimental anthropology and experimental science of religion. Through the use of laboratory methods,30 computer simulations,31 clinical tests,32 and experiments in the field, they are producing groundbreaking results, which should be of interest to all scholars of religion. Leading cognitive scientists of religion such as Scott Atran, Richard Sosis, Joe Henrich, and Ara Norenzayan have pioneered experimental approaches in the field.33 A number of researchers and associates at the Institute of Cognitive and Evolutionary Anthropology in Oxford combine fieldwork methodology with cognitive approaches. Besides Harvey Whitehouse’swork,mentioncanbemadeof EmmaCohen(2007)andCristineH. Legare (Legare and Gelman 2008, 2009). In Aarhus, this interplay has been put into systematic focus in the context of the research coalition called MINDLab 24 See Deacon 1997; Damasio 2000; Barslaou et al. 2003; and Barslaou et al. 2005; and Wilson 2002. 25 See Hutchins 1995 and Lave and Wenger 1991. 26 See Clark 1997; Clark 2008; and Rowlands 2003. 27 See Donald 1991; Donald 2001; Hutchins 1995; and Salomon 1997. 28 See Mithen 1996; Malafouris 2008; Malafouris 2010; Renfrew and Scarre 1998; and Renfrew, Frith and Malafouris 2009. 29 See Deacon 1997; Deacon 2003a; Deacon 2003b; Donald 1991; Donald 2001; and Tomasello 1999. 30 This is by far the most dominant approach. Here is a brief selection of studies: Barrett and Keil 1996; Barrett 2004; Bering et al. 2005; Bering 2006; McKay and Dennett 2009; McKay et al. 2011; Nielbo and Sørensen 2011; Nielbo and Sørensen 2013; Russell et al. 2011; Russell and Gobet 2012; Sibley and Bulbulia 2012; Sousa 2009; Sousa 2010; Sørensen 2007a; Sørensen 2007b; and Sørensen 2013. 31 See Atkinson and Bourrat 2011; Bourrat et al. 2011; Nielbo 2012; Nielbo et al. 2012; Nielbo and Sørensen 2013; Upal 2005; and Upal and Sun 2006. 32 See Allen et al. 2012; Ge et al. 2009; Han et al. 2007; Han et al. 2010; Jegindø et al. 2013; McNamara 2009; Schjoedt 2009; Schjoedt et al. 2008; Schjoedt et al. 2009; Schjoedt et al. 2011; and Vestergaard-Poulsen et al. 2009. 33 See Atran 2010; Henrich and Henrich 2007; Norenzayan and Lee 2010; and Sosis 2008. See also the work of anthropologist Tanya M. Luhrmann (2000, 2011, 2012). 264 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 at Aarhus University.34 Dimitris Xygalatas and William W. McCorkle, Jr. (2010) together with Aleš Chalupa (2011) have established the LEVYNA Lab in Brno dedicated to developing laboratory and fieldwork methodologies. In New Zealand, rcc confederate Joseph Bulbulia and his teams have also produced exciting work in this new field (Fischer et al. 2013). Perhaps the new “neuroanthropology” is promising in this respect. Andreas Roepstorff and Chris Frith, however, argued that the promised methodological and conceptual synthesis offered by neuroanthropology will most likely not bridge the current divides within anthropology. (And, I would argue, other cultural sciences as well.) They suggest that the way forward is “to open up novel ways to do and think ‘experimental anthropology’, as a method, as an object of study and as a research aesthetic” (Roepstorff and Frith 2012:101).35 The most pressing challenge, I think, for all scholars of culture and religion is to take advantage of and gain influence on the growing number of empirical studies demonstrating the way that culture affects and rewires the brain.36 Neurologists have already established a subdiscipline called “social cognitive and affective neuroscience,”37 and, more significantly, a new field called “cultural neuroscience.”38 Happy as we are that our colleagues in the neurosciences have discovered culture, happier we would be if specialists in cultural analysis played a more visible, foundational role. Conclusion Interestingly, today we have come full round to issues that were current in the late nineteenth century. We are struggling once again to maintain the “science” in the comparative science of religion. We are fruitfully enmeshed in interdisciplinarity, multidisciplinarity, and the crucial interchange between the natural, social, and human sciences. Once again, psychology and other behavioral sciences are active, in other guises of course.There is, however, one last topic — 34 Højbjerg2004;Højbjerg2007;Jegindø2012;Konvalinkaetal.2011;Xygalatas2008;Xygalatas 2010; Xygalatas 2012; and Xygalatas et al. 2011. 35 See the special issues in Anthropological Theory 12(1), 2012 (Reyna 2012) and in Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3), 2010 (Chiao 2010). 36 See, for instance, Renfrew, Frith, and Malafouris 2009; Park and Huang 2010. 37 See, for instance, Cacioppo et al. 2002; Cacioppo and Berntson 2005; and the journal by the subdiscipline’s name, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 38 See Chiao 2009 and the special issue of Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (Chiao 2010). 265Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 explicitly present in the nineteenth century — that must be mentioned before closing. And that is evolutionary theory. Evolutionary theory was in fact present throughout the twentieth century in various shades, particularly after the discovery and development of genetics during the 1930s. Evolutionary schemas thrived in sociology, political science, and ethnography up until the middle of the twentieth century. The two world wars crippled the progressive enthusiasm that fed social Darwinism and, as already mentioned, the 1970s changed everything, evolutionary theory consequently falling to the wayside. Thisdidnotlastforlong,however,foralreadyinthelate1980sandearly1990s, the new field of evolutionary psychology entered the scene,39 and, in certain cases, it was formulated explicitly as an alternative to the social sciences, in particular cultural anthropology. The most famous proponents of this alternative were anthropologist John Tooby and psychologist Leda Cosmides in their programmatic essay “The Psychological Foundations of Culture” (1992). They argued that the complexity of human life “is produced by a cognitive architecture, embodied in a physiological system, which interacts with the social and nonsocial world that surrounds it” (Tooby and Cosmides 1992:21) and that the social sciences had attempted to isolate the social from the biological and posit the “blank slate hypothesis” or the “sui generis” understanding of culture, from Durkheim to Kroeber, Boas, Murdock, Lowie, and C. Geertz. Tooby and Cosmides posited the Integrated Causal Model instead of the Standard Social Science Model (sssm). Their claims, I have argued elsewhere, were in certain cases off the mark.40 Nevertheless, they had a significant impact on the first generations of cognitive scientists of religion from the 1990s onwards. The point here is that cognitive approaches were and still are explicitly based on evolutionary theory. But this time, scholars have much more reliable data at hand than in the late nineteenth century. The great advances in geology, biology, genetics, neurology, archaeology, ethology, and comparative psychology during the past decades have significantly changed what we know about the past and how we know it. Based on this new evidence, the cognitive science of religion has attempted to explain how religion arose and why it is still around. The naturalistic approach to religion and to cultural and social evolution has produced important studies by scholars from a variety of disciplines. In 39 See the excellent textbook by Dunbar et al. 2005. 40 See my essay on their misreading of C. Geertz (A. Geertz 2013). For excellent overviews of cognitive theories of the evolution of religion, see Bulbulia 2004; Bulbulia et al. 2008; Slingerland and Bulbulia 2011; and Bulbulia et al. 2013. 266 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 the cognitive science of religion, there are a number of hypotheses that have proven to be robust, such as the counterintuitive ideas hypothesis developed by Pascal Boyer (1994, 2001), the ritual competence hypothesis introduced by E. Thomas Lawson and Robert N. McCauley (Lawson and McCauley 1990; McCauley and Lawson 2002), the costly signaling hypothesis developed by Bulbulia and Sosis (2011), and the modes of religiosity hypothesis developed by HarveyWhitehouse (1995, 2000). Several synthetic volumes, however, have had a much wider impact on the naturalistic study of religion. Evolutionary psychologist and neurologist Merlin Donald was one of the first in recent decades to attempt such a venture in his Origins of the Modern Mind (1991) and revisited in his A Mind So Rare (2001). Since then, anthropologist Roy Rappaport published his Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity (1999), sociologists Jonathan Turner and Alexandra Maryanski published their On the Origin of SocietiesbyNaturalSelection (2008), and sociologist of religion Robert Bellah published his magnum opus Religion in Human Evolution (2011). All of these tomes have formulated frameworks and hypotheses to be pursued by the next generation of scholars. This is truly an exciting time and is reminiscent of the enthusiasm of the late nineteenth century. Let us hope that we have learned the lessons of that era. A significant development is the growing interest by historians of religion to reformulate the history of religions within an evolutionary framework. Some scholars have been influenced by publications other than the above, such as the work of the German Egyptologist Jan Assman (1989, 2000) who based his work on Karl Jaspers (1949). Others have been influenced by the earlier work of S. N. Eisenstadt (1963, 1978, 1986), as evidence by the Jerusalem studies in religion and culture (Arnason et al. 2005). These developments are crucial, I think, to the future of comparative religion. As biologist David Sloan Wilson has often argued, scholars of religion are, in fact, the natural historians of religion, just like the natural historians of biology were in their field (2008:27). Like the situation was in both geology and biology, all that is needed, according to Wilson, is “the transformation of the obvious,” by which he meant the need of a theory (in this case evolutionary theory) to organize the facts that lay before us (ibid.:23). A crucial lacuna in our current state of knowledge is systematic and easily searchable databases containing the cumulative empirical data that archaeologists, historians, linguists, ethnographers, sociologists, and many others have collected during the past few centuries. This is an immense, but highly important, job that needs to be done. Several projects are currently working on exactly that job. The first is the Vancouver based Cultural Evolution of Religion Research Consortium (cerc) that consists of an ethnographic and experimental section and a historical section. The latter consists of database approaches 267Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 to historical areas (Slingerland and Chudek 2011). Peter Turchin (2006), Harvey Whitehouse, Pieter Francois, Edward Slingerland, and Mark Collard are designing and building a database that will cover prehistorical and historical data by regions (Turchin et al. 2012). This project is related to the second historical database project headed by Harvey Whitehouse on the evolution of political systems in collaboration with Camilla Mazzucato, Quentin Atkinson, and Ian Hodder (Whitehouse et al. 2012). On a humorous note, I would like to end this essay with C. Geertz, who wrote that “bringing so large and misshapen a camel as anthropology into psychology’s tent is going to do more to toss things around than to arrange them in order” (1997:197). But that is alright in my opinion. The cognitive science of religion and cultural psychology need tossing around. It makes sense that anthropologists with their holistic approach would make excellent contributions to social psychology and cognitive science. Cognitive scientists of religion tend to downplay culture, which I believe is a mistake. Anthropologists can demonstrate why this is so. Social psychologists are still struggling with trying to bridge the gap between sociology and psychology (Eagly and Fine 2010:313–357). Again, anthropologists and scholars of religion can help pave the way. The challenges that lie ahead seem clear to me: we can either advance our knowledge of humanity or remain stuck in mutual incomprehensibility and interdisciplinary strife. The new generation of scholars and scientists are showing us a way forward. Perhaps the rest of us should follow their lead. References Albinus, Lars. 2005. “Religionsfænomenologi i lyset af den sproglige vending.” In Joel Haviv, Anders Lisdorf, and Peter Weiss Poulsen (eds.), Religionsvidenskabens komparative udfordring, Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press, 19–35. ———. 2010. Religion, magt og kommunikation: Filosofiske overvejelser over religionens betydning i moderniteten, set i krydsfeltet mellem Foucault og Habermas. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press. Allen, Micah, Martin Dietz, Karina S. Blair, Martijn van Beek, Geraint Rees, Peter Vestergaard-Poulsen, Antoine Lutz, and Andreas Roepstorff. 2012. “CognitiveAffective Neural Plasticity Following Active-Controlled Mindfulness Intervention.” The Journal of Neuroscience 32(44):15601–15610. Amundsen, Arne Bugge, ed. 2005. Norges religionshistorie. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget. Antes, Peter, Armin W. Geertz, and Randi Warne, eds. 2004. New Approaches to the Study of Religion. 2 vols. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 268 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Arnason, Johann P., S. N. Eisenstadt, and Björn Wittrock, eds. 2005. Axial Civilizations and World History. Leiden and Boston: Brill. Assmann, Jan. 1989. “Jaspers’ Achsenzeit, oder: Vom Glück und Elend der Zentralperspektive in der Geschichte.” In D. Harth (ed.), Karl Jaspers: Denken zwischen Wissenschaft, Politik und Philosophie, Stuttgart: Metzler, 187–206. ———. 2000. Herrschaft und Heil: PolitischeTheologie in Altägypten, Israel und Europa. München: Beck. Atkinson,QuentinD.,andP.Bourrat.2011.“BeliefsaboutGod,theAfterlifeandMorality Support the Role of Supernatural Policing in Human Cooperation.” Evolution and Human Behavior 32(1):41–49. Atran, Scott. 2010. Talking to the Enemy: Faith, Brotherhood, and the (Un)making of Terrorists. New York: HarperCollins. Barrett, Justin L. 2004. “Bringing Data to Mind: Empirical Claims of Lawson and McCauley’sTheory of Religious Ritual.” InTimothy Light and Brian C.Wilson (eds.), Religion as a Human Capacity: A Festschrift in Honor of E. Thomas Lawson, Leiden: E. J. Brill, 265–288. Barrett, Justin L., and Frank C. Keil. 1996. “Conceptualizing a Nonnatural Entity: Anthropomorphism in God Concepts.” Cognitive Psychology 31(3):219–247. Barsalou, Lawrence W., Aron K. Barbey, W. Kyle Simmons, and Ava Santos. 2005. “Embodiment in Religious Knowledge.” Journal of Cognition and Culture 5(1–2):14–57. Barsalou, Lawrence W., Paula M. Niedenthal, Aron K. Barbey, and Jennifer A. Ruppert. 2003. “Social Embodiment.” The Psychology of Learning and Motivation 43:43–92. Bellah, Robert N. 2011. Religion in Human Evolution: From the Paleolithic to the Axial Age. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press. Bering, Jesse M. 2006. “The Folk Psychology of Souls.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(5):453–498. Bering, Jesse M., Katrina McLeod, and Todd K. Shackelford. 2005. “Reasoning about Dead Agents Reveals Possible Adaptive Trends.” Human Nature 16(4):360–381. Bilde, Per. 1991. “Begrebet religion: Et indlæg i debatten om religionsvidenskabens objekt drøftet i lyset af beslægtede begreber.” Chaos: Dansk-Norsk Tidsskrift for Religionshistoriske Studier 15:4–24. Bourrat, P., Quentin D. Atkinson, and Robin Dunbar. 2011. “Supernatural Punishment and Individual Social Compliance across Cultures.” Religion, Brain and Behavior 1(2):119–134. Boyer, Pascal. 1994. The Naturalness of Religious Ideas: A Cognitive Theory of Religion. Berkeley: University of California Press. ———. 2001. Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. New York: Basic Books. 269Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Buck, Lene, Margrethe Haraldsdatter, Anneline Juul, Charlotte Schönbeck, and Oluf Schönbeck, eds. 1997. Idealer i religion og religionsforskning. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press. Bulbulia, Joseph. 2004. “The Cognitive and Evolutionary Psychology of Religion.” Biology and Philosophy 19(5):655–686. Bulbulia, Joseph, Armin W. Geertz, Quentin D. Atkinson, Emma Cohen, Nicholas Evans, Pieter Francois, Herbert Gintis, Russell D. Gray, Joseph Henrich, Fiona M. Jordon, Ara Norenzayan, Peter J. Richerson, Edward Slingerland, Peter Turchin, Harvey Whitehouse, Thomas Widlok, and David S. Wilson. 2013. “The Cultural Evolution of Religion.” In Peter J. Richerson and Morten H. Christiansen (eds.), Cultural Evolution: Society, Technology, Language, and Religion, Cambridge: mit Press, 381–404. Bulbulia, Joseph, and Richard Sosis. 2011. “Signalling Theory and the Evolution of Religious Cooperation.” Religion 41(3):363–388. Bulbulia, Joseph, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, and Karen Wyman, eds. 2008. The Evolution of Religion: Studies, Theories, and Critiques. Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press. Burkert, Walter. 1972. Homo necans: Interpretationen Altgriechischer Opferriten und Mythen. Berlin: de Gruyter. Peter Bing, trans. 1983. Homo Necans: The Anthropology of Ancient Greek Sacrificial Ritual and Myth. Berkeley: University of California Press. ———. 1996. Creation of the Sacred: Tracks of Biology in Early Religions. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Cacioppo, John T., and Gary G. Berntson, eds. 2005. Social Neuroscience: Key Readings. New York and Hove: Psychology Press. Cacioppo, John T., Gary G. Berntson, Ralph Adolphs, C. Sue Carter, Richard J. Davidson, Martha K. McClintock, Bruce S. McEwen, Michael J. Meaney, Danile L. Schacter, Esther M. Sternberg, Steve S. Suomi, and Shelley E. Taylor, eds. 2002. Foundations in Social Neuroscience. Cambridge: mit Press. Chalupa, Aleš. 2011. “What Might Cognitive Science Contribute to Our Understanding of the Roman Cult of Mithras.” In Luther H. Martin and Jesper Sørensen (eds.), Past Minds: Studies in Cognitive Historiography, London: Equinox Publishing, 107–124. Chiao, Joan Y. 2010. “At the Frontier of Cultural Neuroscience: Introduction to the Special Issue.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3):109–110. ———, ed. 2009. Cultural Neuroscience: Cultural Influences on Brain Function. Amsterdam: Elsevier Press. Clark, Andy. 1997. Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge and London: mit Press. ———. 2008. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. 270 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Cohen, Emma. 2007. The Mind Possessed: The Cognition of Spirit Possession in an AfroBrazilian Religious Tradition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Damasio, Antonio R. 2000. The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London: Heinemann. Deacon, Terrence W. 1997. The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Human Brain. London: Allen Lane The Penguin Press. ———. 2003a. “Multilevel Selection in a Complex Adaptive System: The Problem of Language Origins.” In Bruce H. Weber and David J. Depew (eds.), Evolution and Learning:TheBaldwinEffectReconsidered, Cambridge and London: MIT Press, 81–106. ———. 2003b. “The Hierarchic Logic of Emergence: Untangling the Interdependence of Evolution and Self-Organization.” Bruce H. Weber and David J. Depew (eds.), Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered, Cambridge and London: mit Press, 273–308. Detienne, Marcel. 1981. L’invention de la mythologie. Paris: Gallimard. Margaret Cook, trans. 1986. The Creation of Mythology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Detienne, Marcel, and Jean-Pierre Vernant. 1974. Les ruses de l’intelligence: la métis des Grecs. Paris: Flammarion. Janet Lloyd, trans. 1978. Cunning Intelligence in Greek Culture and Society. Sussex: Harvester Press. Donald, Merlin. 1991. Originsof theModernMind:ThreeStatesintheEvolutionof Culture and Cognition. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press. ———. 2001. A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York and London: W. W. Norton & Company. Dunbar, Robin, Louise Barrett, and John Lycett. 2005. Evolutionary Psychology: A Beginner’s Guide. Human Behaviour, Evolution and the Mind. Oxford: Oneworld. Eagly, Alice, and Gary Alan Fine, eds. 2010. “Bridging Social Psychologies: An Introduction.” Special issue of Social Psychology Quarterly 73(4):313–315. Eisenstadt, S. N. 1963. The Political Systems of Empires. New York: Free Press. ———. 1978. Revolution and the Transformation of Societies: A Comparative Transformation of Societies. New York: Free Press. ———, ed. 1986. The Origin and Diversity of Axial Age Civilizations. Albany: State University of New York Press. Fischer, Ronald, Rohan Callander, Paul Reddish, and Joseph Bulbulia. 2013. “How Do Rituals Affect Cooperation? An Experimental Field Study Comparing Nine Ritual Types.” Human Nature 24(2):115–125. Frisinia, Warren. 1993. “Barbara Deconcini on the aar’s Self-Study.” Religious Studies News 8(3):5. Ge, Jianqiao, Xiasoi Gu, Meng Ji, and Shihui Han. 2009. “Neurocognitive Processes of the Religious Leader in Christians.” Human Brain Mapping 30(12):4012–4024. Geertz, Armin W. 1994a. “Critical Reflections on the Postmodern Study of Religion.” Religion 24(1):16–22. 271Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 ———. 1994b. “On Reciprocity and Mutual Reflection in the Study of Native American Religions.” Religion 24(1):1–7. ———. 1999. “Definition as Analytical Strategy in the Study of Religion.” Historical Reflections/Reflexions Historiques 25(3):445–475. ———. 2003. “Ethnohermeneutics and Worldview Analysis in the Study of Hopi Indian Religion.” Numen 50(3):309–348. ———. 2004a. “Can We Move Beyond Primitivism? On Recovering the Indigenes of Indigenous Religions in the Academic Study of Religion.” In Jacob K. Olupona (ed.), Beyond “Primitivism”: Indigenous Religious Traditions and Modernity, New York: Routledge, 37–70. ———. 2004b. “Cognitive Approaches to the Study of Religion.” In Peter Antes, Armin W. Geertz, and Randi Warne (eds.), New Approaches to the Study of Religion, vol. 2, Textual, Comparative, Sociological, and Cognitive Approaches, Berlin and New York: Walter de Gruyter, 347–399. ———. 2010. “Brain, Body and Culture: A Biocultural Theory of Religion.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 22(4):304–321. ———. 2013. “The Meaningful Brain: Clifford Geertz and the Cognitive Science of Culture.” In Dimitris Xygalatas and William W. McCorkle, Jr. (eds.), Mental Culture: Classical Social Theory and the Cognitive Science of Religion, Durham: Acumen Publishing, 176–196. Geertz, Armin W., and Jeppe Sinding Jensen. 2003. “Religionsteoretisk forskning på InstitutforReligionsvidenskab.”InAndréWangHansen,HelgeKragh,SvendLarsen, Palle Lykke, and Jens Chr. Manniche (eds.), Topforskning ved Aarhus Universitet – en jubilæumsantologi, Aarhus: Aarhus University Press, 325–364. ———, eds. 2011. Religious Narrative, Cognition and Culture: Image and Word in the Mind of Narrative. London: Equinox Publishing. Geertz, Armin W., and Russell T. McCutcheon. 2000. “The Role of Method and Theory in the IAHR.” In Armin W. Geertz, Russell T. McCutcheon, and Scott S. Elliott (eds.), Perspectives on Method and Theory in the Study of Religion, Leiden: E. J. Brill, 3–37. Geertz, Armin W., Russell T. McCutcheon, and Scott S. Elliott, eds. 2000. Perspectives on Method and Theory in the Study of Religion. Leiden: Brill. Geertz, Clifford. 1968. Islam Observed: Religious Development in Morocco and Indonesia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ———. 1984. “Anti Anti-Relativism.” The American Anthropologist 86(2):263–278; reprinted in: Clifford Geertz, Available Light: Anthropological Reflections on Philosophical Topics, Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2000, 42–67. ———. 1995. After the Fact: Two Countries, Four Decades, One Anthropologist. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ———. 1997. “Imbalancing Act: Jerome Bruner’s Cultural Psychology.” In Clifford Geertz, Available Light: Anthropological Reflections on Philosophical Topics, 272 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2000, 187–202; previously published in The New York Review of Books, 10 April 1997 and the whole essay was forthcoming at the time in D. Bakhurst and S. Shanker (eds.), Jerome Bruner: Language, Culture and Self, London: Sage Publications, 2001, 19–30. Gilhus, Ingvild S. 2011. “The Non-Confessional Study of Religion and Its Normative Dimensions.” In Jan-Olav Henriksen (ed.), Difficult Normativity: Normative Dimensions in Research on Religion and Theology, Frankfurt: Peter Lang, 95–107. Girard, René. 1972. La Violence et le Sacré, Paris: Grasset. Patrick Gregory, trans. 1977. Violence and the Sacred. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Guthrie, Stewart Elliott. 1980. “A Cognitive Theory of Religion.” Current Anthropology 21(2):181–203. Hamerton-Kelly, Robert, ed. 1988. Violent Origins: Walter Burkett, René Girard, and Jonathan Z. Smith on Ritual Killing and Cultural Formation. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Hammer, Olav, and James R. Lewis, eds. 2007. The Invention of Sacred Tradition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hammer, Olav, and Kocku von Stuckrad, eds. 2007. Polemical Encounters: Esoteric Discourse and Its Others. Leiden: E. J. Brill. Hammersholt, Torben. 2011. A Critical Analysis of the Comparative Study of Mysticism with Perspectives from Early Buddhism. PhD diss. Odense: University of Southern Denmark. Hammersholt, Torben, and Caroline Schaffalitzky de Muckadell, eds. 2011. At kortlægge religion: grundlagsdiskussioner i religionsforskningen. Højbjerg: Forlaget Univers. Han, Shihui, Xiaosi Gu, Lihua Mao, Jianqiao Ge, GangWang, andYina Ma. 2010. “Neural Substrates of Self-Referential Processing in Chinese Buddhists.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3):332–339. Han, Shihui, Lihua Mao, Xiaosi Gu, Ying Zhu, Jianqiao Ge, and Yina Ma. 2007. “Neural Consequencesof ReligiousBelief onSelf-ReferentialProcessing.”SocialNeuroscience 3(1):1–15. Haviv, Joel, Anders Lisdorf, and Peter Weiss Poulsen, eds. 2005. Religionsvidenskabens komparative udfordring. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanums Press. Henrich, Natalie, and Joseph Henrich. 2007. Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. Højbjerg, Christian Kordt. 2004. “Universalistic Orientations of an Imagistic Mode of Religiosity: The Case of the West African Poro Cult.” In Harvey Whitehouse and James Laidlaw (ed.), Ritual and Memory: Toward a Comparative Anthropology of Religion, Walnut Creek: Altamira Press, 173–185. ———. 2007. Resisting State Iconoclasm among the Loma of Guinea. Durham, n.c.: Carolina Academic Press. 273Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Holland, Dorothy, and Naomi Quinn, eds. 1987. Cultural Models in Language and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Honko, Lauri, ed. 1979. Science of Religion: Studies in Methodology. The Hague: Mouton. ———. 1988. Tradition and Cultural Identity. Turku: Nordic Institute of Folklore. Hultkrantz, Åke. 1973. Metodvägar inom den jämförande religionsforskningen. Stockholm: Esselte Studium ab. Hutchins, Edwin. 1995. Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge and London: mit Press. Jaspers, Karl. 1949. Vom Ursprung und Ziel der Geschichte. Zürich: Artemis-Verlag. Michael Bullock, trans. 1953. The Origin and Goal of History. New Haven: Yale University Press. Jegindø, Else-Marie Elmholdt. 2012. Pain and Coping in the Religious Mind. PhD diss. Aarhus: Aarhus University. Jegindø, Else-Marie Elmholdt, Lene Vase, Joshua Charles Skewes, Astrid Juhl Terkelsen, John Hansen, Armin W. Geertz, Andreas Roepstorff, and Troels Staehelin Jensen. 2013. “Expectations Contribute to Reduced Pain Levels During Prayer in Highly Religious Participants.” Journal of Behavioral Medicine 36(4):413–426. Jensen, Hans Jørgen Lundager. 2000. Den fortærende ild: Strukturelle analyser af narrative og rituelle tekster i Det Gamle Testemente. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press. Jensen, Jeppe Sinding. 1993. “Is a Phenomenology of Religion Possible? On the Ideas of a Human and Social Science of Religion.” Method andTheory in the Study of Religion 5(2):109–133. ———. 1999. “Universals and the Study of Religion.” Historical Reflections/Reflexions Historiques 25(3):423–443. ———. 2002. “The Complex Worlds of Religion: Connecting Cultural and Cognitive Analysis.” InVeikko Anttonen and Ilkka Pyysiäinen (eds.), Current Approaches in the Cognitive Science of Religion, London and New York: Continuum International Publishing Group, 203–225. ———. 2003. The Study of Religion in a New Key: Theoretical and Philosophical Soundings in the Comparative and General Study of Religion. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press. ———. 2010. “Doing It the Other Way Round: Religion as a Basic Case of ‘Normative Cognition.’ ” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 22(4):322–329. Jensen, Jeppe Sinding, and Luther H. Martin, eds. 1997. Rationality and the Study of Religion. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press. Jensen, Tim, and Mikael Rothstein, eds. 2000. Secular Theories on Religion: Current Perspectives. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press. Kamppinen, Matti. 1986. “On the Metaphysics of Cognitivism.” In W. Leinfellner and F. Wuketits (eds.), The Tasks of Contemporary Philosophy, Vienna: Hölder-PichlerTempsky, 313–315. 274 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 ———. 1988. “Homo Religiosus Intelligens: An Outline for a Cognitivist Theory of Religious Behaviour.” Temenos 24:29–38. ———. 1989. Cognitive Systems and Cultural Models of Illness: A Study of Two Mestizo Peasant Communities of the Peruvian Amazon. Helsinki: Academia Scientiarum Fennica. Kamppinen, Matti, ed. 1993. Consciousness, Cognitive Schemata and Relativism. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. Konvalinka, Ivana, Dimitris Xygalatas, Joseph Bulbulia, Uffe Schjødt, Else-Marie Jegindø, S. Wallot, G. Orden, and Andreas Roepstorff. 2011. “Synchronized Arousal Between Performers and Related Spectators in a Fire-Walking Ritual.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108(20):8514–8519. Kraft, Siv Ellen, and Richard J. Natvig, eds. 2006. Metode i religionsvitenskap. Oslo: Pax. Kreinath, Jens, Jan Snoek, and Michael Stausberg, eds. 2006. Theorizing Rituals: Issues, Topics, Approaches, Concepts. Leiden and Boston: Brill Academic Publishers. Lambek, Michael, ed. 2002. A Reader in the Anthropology of Religion. Malden, Oxford, and Carlton: Blackwell Publishing. Lave, Jean, and Etienne Wenger. 1991. Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press. Lawson, E. Thomas, and Robert N. McCauley. 1990. Rethinking Religion: Connecting Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Legare, Cristine H., and S. A. Gelman. 2008. “Bewitchment, Biology, or Both: The Co-existence of Natural and Supernatural Explanatory Frameworks across Development.” Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal 32(4):607–642. ———. 2009. “South African Children’s Understanding of aids and Flu: Investigating Conceptual Understanding of Cause, Treatment, and Prevention.” Journal of Cognition and Culture 9(3):357–370. Luhrmann, Tanya M. 2000. Of Two Minds:The Growing Disorder in American Psychiatry. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. ———. 2011. “Hallucinations and Sensory Overrides.” Annual Review of Anthropology 40:71–85. ———. 2012. When God Talks Back: Understanding the American Evangelical Relationship with God. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Malafouris, Lambros. 2008. “Between Brains, Bodies andThings: Tectonoetic Awareness and the Extended Self.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences 363(1499):1993–2002. ———. 2010. “The Brain-Artefact Interface (bai): A Challenge for Archaeology and Cultural Neuroscience.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3):264–273. Martin, Luther H., and Jesper Sørensen, eds. 2011. Past Minds: Studies in Cognitive Historiography. London: Equinox Publishing. 275Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 McCauley,RobertN.,andE.ThomasLawson.2002.BringingRitualtoMind:Psychological Foundations of Cultural Forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McCorkle, William W., Jr. 2010. Ritualizing the Disposal of the Deceased: From Corpse to Concept. New York: Peter Lang. McKay,Ryan,andDanielDennett.2009.“OurEvolvingBeliefsaboutEvolvedMisbelief.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(6):541–551. McKay, Ryan, C. Efferson, Harvey Whitehouse, and Ernst Fehr. 2011. “Wrath of God: Religious Primes and Punishment.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278(1713):1858–1863. McNamara, Patrick. 2009. The Neuroscience of Religious Experience. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press. Mithen, Steven. 1996. The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London: Thames and Hudson. Nielbo, Kristoffer L. 2012. SpontaneousandHierarchicalSegmentationof Non-functional Events. PhD diss. Aarhus: Aarhus University. Nielbo, Kristoffer L., Donald M. Braxton, and Afzal Upal. 2012. “Computing Religion: A New Tool in the Multilevel Analysis of Religion.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 24(3):267–290. Nielbo, Kristoffer L., and Jesper Sørensen. 2011. “Spontaneous Processing of Functional and Non-functional Action Sequences.” Religion, Brain and Behavior 1(1):18–30. ———. 2013. “Prediction Error During Functional and Non-Functional Action Sequences:AComputationalExplorationof RitualandRitualizedEventProcessing.” Journal of Cognition and Culture 13(3–4):347–365. Norenzayan, Ara, and Albert Lee. 2010. “It Was Meant to Happen: Explaining Cultural Variations in Fate Attributions.” Journalof PersonalityandSocialPsychology 98:702–720. Park, Denise C., and Chih-Mao Huang. 2010. “Culture Wires the Brain: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective.” Perspectives on Psychological Science 5(4):391–400. Petersen, Anders Klostergaard. 2011. “Rituals of Purification, Rituals of Initiation: Phenomenological, Taxonomical and Culturally Evolutionary Reflections.” In David Hellholm, Tor Vegge, Øyvind Nordeval, and Christer Hellholm (eds.), Ablution, Initiation, and Baptism: Late Antiquity, Early Judaism, and Early Christianity, Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, vol. 1, 3–40. ———. 2013. “Paul and Magic: Complementary or Incongruent Entities? An Essay on the Scholarly Use of the Concept of Magic, Its Definition, and the Prevalence of the Phenomenon in Paul’s World.” In Helen Jacobus, Anne K. Gudme, and Philippe Guillaume (eds.), Magic and Divination in the BiblicalWorld, Piscataway, New Jersey: Gorgias Press, 199–228. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. 2001. HowReligionWorks:TowardsaNewCognitiveScienceof Religion. Leiden, Boston, and Köln: E. J. Brill. 276 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Pyysiäinen, Ilkka, and Veikko Anttonen, eds. 2002. Current Approaches in the Cognitive Science of Religion. London and New York: Continuum. Rappaport, Roy A. 1999. Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Renfrew, Colin, and Chris Scarre, eds. 1998. Cognition and Material Culture: The Archaeologyof SymbolicStorage.Cambridge:McDonaldInstituteforArchaeological Research. Renfrew, Colin, Chris Frith, and Lambros Malafouris, eds. 2009. The Sapient Mind: Archaeology Meets Neuroscience. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. Reyna, Stephen P. 2012. “Introduction.” Anthropological Theory 12(1):3–4. Roepstorff, Andreas, and Chris Frith. 2012. “Neuroanthropology or Simply Anthropology? Going Experimental as Method, as Object of Study, and as Research Aesthetic.” Anthropological Theory 12(1):101–111. Rowlands, Mark. 2003. Externalism: Putting Mind and World Back Together Again. Montreal, Kingston, and Ithaca: McGill-Queen’s University Press. Russell, Yvan I., Robin I. M. Dunbar, and F. Gobet. 2011. “Euphoria versus Dysphoria: Differential Cognitive Roles in Religion?” In S. Masmoudi, A. Naceur, and D. Y. Dai (eds.), Attention, Representation and Performance: Integration of Cognition, Emotion and Motivation, New York: Psychology Press, 147–165. Russell, Yvan I., and F. Gobet. 2012. “Sinuosity and the Affect Grid: A Method for Adjusting Repeated Mood Scores.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 114:125–136. Saler, Benson. 2009. Understanding Religion: Selected Essays. Berlin and New York: Walter de Gruyter. Salomon, Gavriel. 1997. Distributed Cognitions: Psychological and Educational Considerations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sand, Erik Reenberg, and Jørgen Podemann Sørensen, eds. 1999. ComparativeStudiesin History of Religions: Their Aim, Scope, and Validity. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press. Schjødt, Jens Peter. 2008. Initiation Between Two Worlds: Structure and Symbolism in Pre-ChristianScandinavianReligion.Odense:UniversityPressof SouthernDenmark. Schjoedt, Uffe. 2009. “The Religious Brain: A General Introduction to the Experimental Neuroscience of Religion.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 21(3):310–339. Schjoedt, Uffe, Hans Stødkilde-Jørgensen, Armin W. Geertz, Torben E. Lund, and Andreas Roepstorff. 2011. “The Power of Charisma: Perceived Charisma Inhibits the Frontal Executive Network of Believers in Intercessory Prayer.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6(1):119–127. Schjoedt, Uffe, Hans Stødkilde-Jørgensen, Armin W. Geertz, and Andreas Roepstorff. 2008. “Rewarding Prayers.” Neuroscience Letters 443(3):165–168. 277Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 ———. 2009. “Highly Religious Participants Recruit Areas of Social Cognition in Personal Prayer.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4(2):199–207. Sibley, C. G., and Joseph Bulbulia. 2012. “Healing Those Who Need Healing: How Religious Practice Interacts with Personality to Affect Social Belonging.” Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion 1(1):29–45. Slingerland, Edward, and Joseph Bulbulia. 2011. “Introductory Essay: Evolutionary Science and the Study of Religion.” Religion 41(3):307–328. Slingerland, Edward, and Maciej Chudek. 2011. “The Prevalence of Mind-Body Dualism in Early China.” Cognitive Science. A Multidisciplinary Journal 35(5):997–1007. Smith, Jonathan Z. 1978. Map Is NotTerritory: Studies in the History of Religions. Leiden: E. J. Brill. Sørensen, Jesper. 2007a. A Cognitive Theory of Magic. Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press. ———. 2007b. “Acts that Work: A Cognitive Approach to Ritual Agency.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 19(3–4):281–300. ———. 2013. “Magic Reconsidered: Towards a Scientifically Valid Concept of Magic.” In Michael Stausberg and Bernd-Christian Otto (eds.), Defining Magic: A Reader, London: Equinox Publishing, 229–242. Sosis, Richard. 2008. “Pigeons, Foxholes, and the Book of Psalms: Evolved Superstitious ResponsestoCopewithStressandUncertainty.”InJosephBulbulia,RichardSosis,Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, and KarenWyman (eds.), TheEvolutionof Religion: Studies,Theories,andCritiques, Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press, 103–109. Sousa, Paulo. 2009. “On Testing the ‘Moral Law.’ ” Mind and Language 24(2):209–234. ———. 2010. “Folk Concepts of Intentional Action in Contexts of Amoral and Immoral Luck.” Review of Philosophy and Psychology 1(3):351–370. Sperber, Dan. 1974. Le symbolisme en général. Paris: Hermann. Alice L. Morton, trans. 1975. Rethinking Symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ———. 1982. Le savoir des anthropologues. Paris: Hermann. Anonymous, trans. 1985. On Anthropological Knowledge [Cambridge Studies in Social Anthropology 54]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sperber, Dan, and Deirdre Wilson. 1986. Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. Stausberg, Michael, ed. 2009. Contemporary Theories of Religion: A Critical Companion. Abingdon and New York: Routledge. Stausberg, Michael, and Steven Engler, eds. 2011. The Routledge Handbook of Research Methods in the Study of Religion. Abingdon and New York: Routledge. Taves, Ann. 1999. Fits, Trances and Visions: Experiencing Religion and Explaining Experience from Wesley to James. Princeton: Princeton University Press. ———. 2009. Religious Experience Reconsidered: A Building Block Approach to the Study of Religion and Other Special Things. Princeton: Princeton University Press. 278 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 ———. 2010. “No Field Is an Island: Fostering Collaboration Between the Academic Study of Religion and the Sciences.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 22:170–188. ———. 2011. “‘Religion’ in the Humanities and the Humanities in the University.” Presidential Plenary Address, American Academy of Religion, 30 October 2010. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 79(2):287–314. Thagard, Paul. 1996. Mind: Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge and London: mit Press. ———. 2010. The Brain and the Meaning of Life. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press. Thomassen, Einar. 2007. “Tekstkritikk og kanon.” In Odd Einar Haugen, Christian Janss, and Tone Modalsli (eds.), Filologi og hermeneutikk. Bidrag til ein konferanse halden av Nordisk Nettverk for Edisjonsfilologar 16.–18. september 2005, Oslo: Solum, 142–157. Tomasello, Michael. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press. Tooby, John, and Leda Cosmides. 1992. “The Psychological Foundations of Culture.” In Jerome H. Barkow, Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby (eds.), The Adapted Mind: EvolutionaryPsychologyandtheGenerationof Culture, Oxford and NewYork: Oxford University Press, 19–136. Turchin, Peter. 2006. War and Peace and War: The Rise and Fall of Empires. New York: Pi Press. Turchin, Peter, Harvey Whitehouse, Pieter Francois, Edward Slingerland, and Mark Collard. 2012. “A Historical Database of Sociocultural Evolution.” Cliodynamics: The Journal of Theoretical and Mathematical History 3(2):271–293. Turner, Jonathan H., and Alexandra Maryanski. 2008. On the Origin of Societies by Natural Selection. Boulder and London: Paradigm Publishers. Tybjerg, Tove. 2010. Religionsforskningen før og nu: Historiske rødder. Copenhagen: Gyldendalske Boghandel, Nordisk Forlag. Tyler,Stephen,ed.1969.CognitiveAnthropology.NewYork:Holt,RinehartandWinston. Upal, M. Afzal. 2005. “Simulating the Emergence of New Religious Movements.” Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 8(1). url: http://jasss.soc.surrey.ac. uk/8/1/6.html (accessed 17 December 2013). Upal, M. Afzal, and R. Sun, eds. 2006. Cognitive Modeling and Agent-based Social Simulation: Papers from the aaai Workshop. Menlo Park: aaai Press. Vernant,Jean-Pierre.1962.Lesoriginesdelapenséegrecque.Paris:PressesUniversitaires de France. Anonymous, trans. 1982. TheOriginsof GreekThought. London: Methuen. ———. 1965. Mythe et pensée chez les Grecs: Etudes de psychologie historique. Paris: La Decouverte. Janet Lloyd, trans., with J. Fort. 1983. Myth and Thought among the Greeks. Cambridge: Zone Books. 279Long-lost Brothers Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Vestergaard-Poulsen,Peter,MartijnvanBeek,JoshuaSkewes,CarstenBjarkam,Michael Stubberup, Jes Bertelsen, and Andreas Roepstorff. 2009. “Long-term Meditation Is Associated with Increased Gray Matter Density in the Brain Stem.” NeuroReport 20(2):170–174. Waardenburg, Jacques, ed. 1973–1974. Classical Approaches to the Study of Religion: Aims, Methods and Theories of Research. 2 vols. The Hague: Mouton. Warburg, Margit. 2006. Citizens of theWorld: A History and Sociology of the Baha’is from a Globalisation Perspective. Leiden: E. J. Brill. Whaling, Frank, ed. 1983–1985. Contemporary Approaches to the Study of Religion. 2 vols. Berlin: Mouton. Whitehouse, Harvey. 1995. Inside the Cult: Religious Innovation and Transmission in Papua New Guinea. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ———. 2000. Arguments and Icons: Divergent Modes of Religiosity. London and New York: Oxford University Press. Whitehouse, Harvey, Ken Kahn, Michael E. Hochberg, and Joanna J. Bryson. 2012. “The Role of Simulations in Theory Construction for the Social Sciences: Case Studies Concerning Divergent Modes of Religiosity.” Religion, Brain and Behavior 2(3):182–201. Wiebe, Donald. 1999. The Politics of Religious Studies: The Continuing Conflict with Theology in the Academy. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Wilson, David Sloan. 2008. “Evolution and Religion: The Transformation of the Obvious.” In Joseph Bulbulia, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, and Karen Wyman (eds.), The Evolution of Religion: Studies, Theories, and Critiques, Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press, 23–29. Wilson, Margaret. 2002. “Six Views of Embodied Cognition.” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9(4):625–636. Wulff, David M. 2003. “A Field in Crisis: Is It Time for the Psychology of Religion to Start Over?” In Peter H. M. P. Roelofsma, Jozef M. T. Corveleyn, and Joke W. van Saane (eds.), One Hundred Years of Psychology and Religion: Issues and Trends in a Century Long Quest, Amsterdam: Vrije University Press, 11–32. Xygalatas, Dimitris. 2008. “Firewalking and the Brain: The Physiology of High-Arousal Rituals.” In Joseph Bulbulia, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, and Karen Wyman (eds.), The Evolution of Religion: Studies, Theories, and Critiques, Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press, 189–195. ———. 2010. “Can the Study of Religion Be Scientific?” In Panayotis Pachis and Donald Wiebe (eds.), Chasing Down Religion: In the Sights of History and the Cognitive Sciences. Essays in Honor of Luther H. Martin, Thessaloniki: Barbounakis Publications, 531–549. ———. 2012. The Burning Saints: Cognition and Culture in the Fire-Walking Rituals of the Anastenaria. Sheffield: Equinox Publishing. 280 Geertz Numen 61 (2014) 255–280 Xygalatas, Dimitris, Ivana Konvalinka, Andreas Roepstorff, and Joseph Bulbulia. 2011. “Quantifying Collective Effervescence: Heart-Rate Dynamics at a Fire-Walking Ritual.” Communicative and Integrative Biology 4(6):735–738.