Přepnout na evidenci publikací, které mě citují.
HRBKOVÁ, Lenka. Understanding Policy Attitudes: Effect of Affective Source Cues on Political Reasoning. 1. vyd. Brno: Masarykova univerzita, 2016, 173 s. ISBN 978-80-210-8056-0. Dostupné z: https://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CZ.MUNI.M210-8056-2016.
Bibliografie
Abelson, R. (1963). Computer Simulation of Hot Cognition. In S. Tomkins, & S. Messick (Eds.), Computer Simulation of Personality: Frontier of Psychological Theory (pp. 277-298). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Achen, C. H. (1975). Mass Political Attittudes and the Survey Response. The American Political Science Review, 69(4), 1218-1231.A DOI: 10.2307/1955282
Althaus, S. L. (1998). Information Effects in Collective Preferences. The American Political Science Review, 92(3), 545-558. DOI: 10.2307/2585480
Andersen, R., Tilley, J., & Heath, A. (2005). Political Knowledge and Enlightened Preferences: Party Choice through the Electoral Cycle. British Journal of Political Science, 35(2), 285-302.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. DOI: 10.1017/S0007123405000153
Arceneaux, K. (2008). Can Partisan Cues Diminish Democratic Accountability? Political Behavior, 30(2), 139-160. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-007-9044-7
Arceneaux, K., & Kolodny, R. (2009). Educating the Least Informed: Group Endorsement in a Grasroot Campaign. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 755-770. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2009.00399.x
Aronson, A. (2013). Journal of Experimental Political Science. http://themonkeycage.org/2013/07/journal-of-experimental-political-science/
Aronson, E., Brewer, M. B., & Carlsmith, M. J. (1985). In L. Gardner, & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology. 3rd Edition (pp. 441-486). New York: Random House.
Aronson, E., Brewer, M., & Carlsmith, M. (1985). “Experimentation in Social Psychology.” In L. Gardner & E: Aronson (eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology 2nd Edition. Reading: Addison-Wesley.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence, & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 2. (pp. 89-195). London: Academic Press.
Bargh, J. (1997). The Automaticity of Everyday Life. In R. Wyer (Ed.), Advances of Social Cognition (pp. 1-61). Mahwan: Erlbaum.
Bargh, J. (2007). Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20-46. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2
Bartels, L. M. (1996). Uninformed Votes: Information Effects in Presidential Elections. American Journal of Political Science, 40(1), 194-230. DOI: 10.2307/2111700
Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117-150. DOI: 10.1023/A:1021226224601
Bassi, A. (2006). Experiments on Approval Voting. Working paper, New York University.
Baumeister, A. T. (2001). Bad Is Stronger than Good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
Baxter, B. W., & Hinson, R. E. (2001). Is smoking automatic? Demands of smoking behavior on attentional resources. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 59-66. DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.59
Bayes, T. (1763). An Essay towards Solving a Problem in the Doctrine of Chances. Philosophical Transactions, 53, 370-418.
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55, 30-40. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001
Bechara, A. et al. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215-225. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215
Bechara, A. et al. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1294. DOI: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293
Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954) Voting: A Study of Opinion Formation in a Presidential Campaign. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Bernoulli, D. (1954). Exposition of a new theory on the measurement of risk. Econometrica, 22, 23-36 (Translation of Bernoulli, D. [1738]. Specimen theoriae novae de mensura sortis. Papers Imp. Acad. Sci. St. Petersburg, 5, 175-192). DOI: 10.2307/1909829
Bittner, A. (2015). Leader Evaluation and Partisan Stereotypes - A Comparative Analysis. In M. C. Lobo, & J. Curtice (Eds.), Personality Politics? The Role of Leader Evaluations in Democratic Elections (pp. 17-37). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Blondel, J. et al. (2010). Political Leadership, Parties and Citizens. The Personalization of Leadership. Abingdon: Routledge.
Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., & Cook, F. L. (2013). The Influence of Partisan Motivated Reasoning on Public Opinion. Political Behavior, 36: 235-262. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-013-9238-0
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265-289.
Boudreau, C. (2009). Closing the gap: When do cues eliminate differences sophisticated and unsophisticated citizens? Journal of Politics, 7, 964-975. DOI: 10.1017/S0022381609090823
Box, G. E. P., Hunter W. G., & Hunter, S. J. (1978). Statistics for Experimenters. And Introduction to Design, Data, Analysis and Model Building. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Brader, T. (2006). Campaigning for Hearts and Minds. How Emotional Appeals in Political Ads Work. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Brader, T. (2011). The Political Relevance of Emotions: “Reassessing” Revisited. Political Psychology, 32(2), 337-346.
Brader, T. A. & Tucker, J. A. (2012a). Following the Party’s Lead: Party Cues, Policy Opinion, and the Power of Partisanship in Three Multiparty Systems. Comparative Politics, 44(4), 403-420. DOI: 10.5129/001041512801283004
Brader, T. A., & Tucker, J. A. (2009). What’s Left behind When the Party’s Over: Survey Experiments on the Effects of Partisan Cues in Putin’s Russia. Politics and Policy, 37(4), 843-868.
Brader, T. A., & Tucker, J. A. (2012). Survey Experiments: Partisan Cues in Multi-Party Systems. In B. Kittel, W. J. Luhan, & R. B. Morton (Eds.), Experimental Political Science. Principles and Practices (pp. 112-138). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Brader, T. A., Duell, D. & Tucker, J. A. (2013). Which Parties Can Lead Opinion? Experimental Evidence on Partisan Cue Taking in Multiparty Democracies. Comparative Political Studies, 46, 1485-1517. DOI: 10.1177/0010414012453452
Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What Triggers Public Opposition to Immigration? Anxiety, Group Cues, and Immigration Threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52 (4), 959–978. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00353.x
Brady, H. E., & Sniderman, P. M. (1985). Attitude Attribution: A Group Basis for Political Reasoning. The American Political Science Review, 79(4), 1061-1078. DOI: 10.2307/1956248
Bullock, J. G. (2011). Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. American Political Science Review, 105, 496-515. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055411000165
Burdein, I., Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2006). Experiments on the Automaticity of Political Beliefs and Attitudes. Political Psychology, 27, 359-371. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00504.x
Cacioppo, J. T. et al. (1986). Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: An individual difference perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1032-1043. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.5.1032
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L, & Bertson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures. The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1(1), 3-25. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_2
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L, & Bertson, G. G. (1999). The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 8, 133-137.
Cacioppo, T. J., & Petty, R. E. (1979). Effects of Message Repetition and Position on Cognitive Response, Recall, and Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 97-109.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American Voter. Chicago: The University Chicago Press.
Campbell, D. T. (1957). Factors relevant to the validity of experiments in social settings. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 297–312. DOI: 10.1037/h0040950
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Capelos, T. (2010). Feeling the Issues: How Citzens’ Affective Reactions and Leadership Perception Shape Policy Evaluations. Journal of Political Marketing, 9(1), 9-33. DOI: 10.1080/15377850903583038
Caplan, B. (2007). The Myth of the Rational Voter. Princeton: Priceton University Press.
Caprara, G. V., and Vecchione, M. (2013). Personality Approaches to Political Behavior. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology (pp. 23-58). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1986). Trait Memory and Behavior Memory: The Effects of Alternative Pathways on Impression Judgment Response Times. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 50(1), 5-13. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.1.5
Carmines, E. G. & Kuklinski, J. H. (1990). Incentives, Opportunities, and the Logic of public Opinion in American Political Representation. In J. A. Ferejohn & J. H. Kuklinski (eds.), Information and Democratic Processes. Urban: University of Illinois Press.
Carsey, T. M., & Layman, G. C. (2006). Changing Sides of Changing Minds: Party Identification and Policy Preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 464-477. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00196.x
Cassino, D., & Lodge, M. (2007). The Primacy of Affect in Political Evaluation. In R. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior (pp. 101-123). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Cassino, D., Lodge, M. & Taber, C. S. (2014). Implicit Political Attitudes: When, How, Why, With What Effects? In K. Kenski, & K. H. Jamieson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Communication. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over Policy: The Dominating Impact of Group Influence on Political Beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808-822. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.808
Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1989) Candidate Perception in an Ambiguous World: Campaigns, Cues, and Inferences Processes. American Journal of Political Science, 33(4), 912-940. DOI: 10.2307/2111115
Converse, P. E. (1964). The Nature of Belief System in mass Public. In D.E. Apter (Ed.). Ideology and Discontent. (pp. 206-261), New York: Free Press.
Cover, A. D., & Brumberg, B. S. (1982). Baby Books and Ballots: The Impact of Congressional Mail on Constituent Opinion. American Political Science Review, 76, 347-59. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055400187040
Curtice, J. (2003). Elections as Beauty Contests: Do the Rules Matter? Paper presented at the International Conference, Lisbon.
Curtice, J., & Blais, A. (2001). Follow My Leader? A Cross-National Analysis of Leadership Effects in Parliamentary Democracies. Working paper, University of Oxford.
Curtice, J., & Holmberg, S. (2005). Party leaders and party choice. In J. Thomassen (Ed.), The European Voter (pp. 235-254). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Curtice, J., & Lisi, M. (2013). The Impact of Leaders in Presidential and Parliamentary Regimes. In M. C. Lobo, & J. Curtice (Eds.), Personality Politics? The Role of Leader Evaluations in Democratic Elections (pp. 63-86). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dakhlia, J. (2010). Politique People. Paris: Edition Breal.
Dalton, R. J. (2002). The Decline of Party Identification. In R.J. Dalton, & M.P. Wattenberg (Eds.), Parties without Partisans: Political Change in Advanced Industrial Democracies (pp. 19-36). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Avon.
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 351, 1413-1420. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125
Delli Caprini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1991). Stability and Change in the U.S. Public’s Knowledge of Politics. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 583-612. DOI: 10.1086/269283
Delli Caprini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans Know About Politics And Why It Matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Demertzis, N. (2006). The Affect Dimension in Political Tension. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Dickson, E., & Scheve, K. (2006). Testing the Effects of Social Identity Appeals in Election Campaigns: An fMRI Study. Working paper, Yale University.
Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. Boston: Addison-Wesley.
Druckman, J. N. (2001). The Implications of Framing Effects for Citizen Competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 225-256. DOI: 10.1023/A:1015006907312
Druckman, J. N. et al. (2006). The Growth and Development of Experimental Research in Political Science. American Political Science Review, 4, 627- 635. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055406062514
Druckman, J. N. et al. (2011). Experimentation in Political Science. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science (pp. 3-14). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Druckman, J. N., & Kam, C. D. (2011). Students as Experimental Participants: A Defense of the “Narrow Data Base“. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science (pp. 41-57). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Druckman, J. N., & McDermott, R. (2008). Emotion and the Framing of Risky Choice. Political Behavior, 30, 297-321. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-008-9056-y
Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How Elite Partisan Polarization Affects Public Opinion Formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57-79. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055412000500
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Eibl, O., Chytilek, R., & Linek, L. (Forthcoming). Obrana, akvizice či nesoutěž? Jak se měnila podoba a intenzita soutěže mezi českými stranami v letech 2006-2014? Sociologický časopis / Czech Sociological Review.
Evans, J. S. T. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-Process Theories of Higher Cognition: Advancing the Debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223–241.
Fazio, R. et al. (1986). On the Automatic Activation of Attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229-238. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In C. Hendrick, & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Research Methods in Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 74-97). London: Sage Publications.
Feldman, S., & Conover, P. J. (1983). Candidates, Issues and Voters: The Role of Inference in Political Perception. The Journal of Politics, 455(4), 810-839. DOI: 10.2307/2130414
Feldman-Barett, L. (2015). Psychology is not in crisis. The New York Times Available at: http://www.nytimes.com/2015/09/01/opinion/psychology-is-not-in-crisis.html?_r=0
Festinger, L. (1975). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective Voting in American Elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Fischle, M. (2000). Mass Response to the Lewinsky Scandal: Motivated Reasoning or Bayesian Updating? Political Psychology, 21(1), 135-159.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. S. (1991). Social Cognition. 2nd Ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fiske, T. S. (1980). Attention and Weight in Person Perception: The Impact of Negative and Extreme Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 889-906. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.6.889
Fiske, T. S., et al. (2002). A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Statis and Competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878-902. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878
Flanagan, M. F., & Dipboye, R. L. (1980). Representativeness does have implication for the generalizability of laboratory and field research findings. American Psychologist, 35, 464-467. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.35.5.464
Funk, C. (1997). Implications of Political Expertise in Candidate Trait Evaluations. Political Research Quarterly, 50(3), 675-697. DOI: 10.1177/106591299705000309
Garzia, D. (2011). The Personalization of Politics in the Western Democracies. Causes and Consequences on Leader-Follower Relationships. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 697-709. DOI: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.05.010
Garzia, D. (2012). The Rise of Party/Leader Identification in Western Europe. Political Research Quarterly, 20, 1-12.
Garzia, D. (2013). Changing Parties, Changing Partisans: The Personalization of partisan Attachments in Western Europe. Political Psychology, 34(1), 67-89. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012.00918.x
Garzia, D. (2013). Personalization of Politics and Electoral Change in Western Europe. Paper presented at the Western Political Science Association Annual Meeting.
Gelman, A. (2014). The connection between varying treatment effects and the crisis of unreplicable research: A Bayesian perspective. Journal of Management, 41(2), 632-643.
Gerber, A., & Green, D. P. (1998). Rational Learning and Partisan Attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 42(3), 794-818. DOI: 10.2307/2991730
Gigerenzer, G. et al. (1999). Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650-669. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650
Gigerenzer, G., & Stelten, R. (2001). Rethinking Rationality. In G. Gigerenzer, & R. Stelten (Eds.), Bounded Rationality: the Adaptive Toolbox (pp. 1-12). Cambridge: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.
Gigerenzer, G., Czerlinski, J., & Martignon, L. (2002). How good are fast and frugal heuristics? In T. Gilovich, & D. Griffin (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment; heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. (pp. 559-581). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phase and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 53-92). New York: Guilford.
Goren, P. (2005). Party Identification and Key Political Values. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 881-896. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2005.00161.x
Goren, P., Federico, Ch. M., & Kitilson, M. C. (2009). Source Cues, Partisan Identities, and Political Value Expression. American Journal of Political Science, 55, 805-20.
Gosnell, Harold F. (1926). An Experiment in the Stimulation of Voting. American Political Science Review, 20, 869–874. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055400110524
Grant, J. T. et al. (1999). The Accessibility of Party Identification during a Political Campaign Season. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Association for Public Opinion Research.
Green, D. P., & Gerber, A. S. (2003). The Underprovision of Experiments in Political Science. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 589, 94-112. DOI: 10.1177/0002716203254763
Greenwald,A., & Banaji, M. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitude, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Political Review, 102(1), 814-136.
Greitemeyer, T. et al. (2009). Biased assimilation: The role of source position. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 22-39. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.497
Groenendyk, E. W. (2013). Competing Motives in the Partisan Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gschwend, T. (2005). Analyzing Quota Sample Data and the Peer-review Process. French Politics, 3, 99-91. DOI: 10.1057/palgrave.fp.8200068
Guala, F. (2003). Experimental Localism and External Validity. Philosophy of Science, 70, 1195-1205. DOI: 10.1086/377400
Hájek, R. (2012). Personalizace, estetizace a depolitizace politiky v českých médiích: Mediální pokrytí sjezdů ČSSD a kongresů ODS mezi lety 1994-2011 v denících Právo a Mladá fronta Dnes. Mediální studia, 1, 76-93.
Hanley, S. (2011). Dynamika utváření nových stran v České republice v letech 1996-2010: hledání možných příčin politického zemětřesení. Sociologický časopis / Czech Sociological Review, 47(1), 115-136.
Hastie, R. (1986). A primer of information-processing theory for the political scientist. In R.R. Lau, & D. O Sears (Eds.), Political Cognition: The nineteenth Carnegie annual Symposium on Cognition, (pp. 11-39). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Hayter, A. J. (1986). The maximum familywise type I error rate of Fisher’s least significant difference test. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81(396), 1000-1004. DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1986.10478364
Henry, P. J. (2008). College Sophomores in the Laboratory Redux: Influences of a Narrow Data Base on Social Psychology’s View of the Nature of Prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 19 (2), 49-71. DOI: 10.1080/10478400802049936
Henry, P. J. (2008). Student Sampling as a Theoretical Problem. Psychological Inquiry, 19 (2), 114-126. DOI: 10.1080/10478400802049951
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underline variations in political ideology. Behavioral Brain Sicence, 37(3), 297-307. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X13001192
Hloušek, V., & Kopeček, L. (2005). Cleavages in Contemporary Czech and Slovak Politics: between Persistence and Change. Presented at the ECPR Conference, Budapest, 8-10 September 2005.
Hoaglin, D. C., & Iglewicz, B. (1987). Fine tuning some resistant rules for outlier labeling. Journal of American Statistical Association, 82, 1147-1149. DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1987.10478551
Hoaglin, D. C., Iglewicz, B., & Tukey, J.W. (1986). Performance of some resistant rules for outlier labeling. Journal of American Statistical Association, 81, 991-999. DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1986.10478363
Holbrook, A. L. (2011). Attitude Change Experiments in Political Science. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science (pp. 141-154). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Holtz-Bacha, C. (2004). Germany: How private life of politicians got into the media. Parliamentary Affairs, 57(1), 41-52. DOI: 10.1093/pa/gsh004
Hooghe et al. (2012). Gathering Counter-Factual Evidence: An Experimental Study on Voters’ Responses. In B. Kittel, W. J. Luhan, & R. B. Morton (Eds.), Experimental Political Science: Principles and Practices (pp. 233-248). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Hsee, Ch. K. (1995). Elastic justification: How tempting but task irrelevant factors influence decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 330-337. DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1995.1054
Hsee, Ch. K. (1998). Less is better: When low-value options are valued more highly then high value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11, 107-121. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199806)11:2<107::AID-BDM292>3.0.CO;2-Y
Huckfeldt, R. et al. (1999). Accessibility and the Political Utility of Partisan and Ideological Orientations. American Journal of Political Science, 43, 888-911. DOI: 10.2307/2991839
Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (2000). Political Consequences of Inconsistency: The Accessibility and Stability of Abortion Attitudes. Political Psychology, 21, 57-79. DOI: 10.1111/0162-895X.00177
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., & Cassese, E. (2006). On the Distinct Political Effects of Anxiety and Anger. In A. Crigler, M. MacKuen, G. E. Marcus, & W. R. Neuman (Eds.), The Political Dynamics of Feeling and Thinking. The University of Press.
Imai, K., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2013). Experimental designs for identifying causal mechanism. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 176, 5-51. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-985X.2012.01032.x
Jackson, M., & Cox, D. R. (2013). The Principles of Experimental Design and Their Application in Sociology. Annual Review of Sociology, 39 (1), 27-49. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-soc-071811-145443
Jacobson, G. C. (1992). The Politics of Congressional Elections. New York: Harper Collins
Jacoby, W. G. (1988). The Impact of Party Identification on Issue Attitudes. American journal of Political Science, 32(3), 643-661-Jimenez-Buedo, M., & Miller, L. M. (2010). Why a Trade-Off? The Relationship between the External and Internal Validity of Experiments. Theoria, 69, 301-321. DOI: 10.2307/2111240
Jurek P. (2012) Personalizace volebního systému - případ Poslanecké sněmovny Parlamentu ČR. Politics in Central Europe, 8(2), 124-140.
Kahan, D. M. (2013). Ideology, Motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(4), 407-424.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Myšlení rychlé a pomalé. Brno: Jan Melvil.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-292. DOI: 10.2307/1914185
Kalkhoff, W. et al. (2007). Human Participants in Laboratory Experiments in the Social Sciences. In M. Webster, & J. Sell (Eds.). Laboratory Experiments in Social Sciences (pp. 243-263). Burlington: Elsevier.
Kam, C. D. (2005). Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and Individual Differences. Political Behavior, 27(2), 163-182. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-005-1764-y
Kam, C. D. (2007). When Duty Calls, Do Citizens Answer? Journal of Politics, 69, 17–29. DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00491.x
Kam, C. D. et al. (2007). Beyond the “Narrow Data Base“: Another Convenience Sample for Experimental Research. Political Behavior, 29 (4), 415-440. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-007-9037-6
Karvonen, L. (2010). The Personalization of Politics. A Study of Parliamentary Democracies. Colchester: ECPR Press.
Key, V. O. (1996). The Responsible Electorate: Rationality in Presidential Voting 1936-1960. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Kinder, D. (1986). Presidential character revisited. In R. Lau, & D. Sears (Eds.), Political cognition (pp. 235-255). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1968). Unhappy memories of the “happy past”: Studies in cognitive dissonance. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 157-166. DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1968.tb01128.x
Kriesi, H. (2011). Personalization of national election campaign. Party Politics, 1-20.
Krosnick, J. A. (1988). The role of attitude importance in social evaluation: A study of policy preferences, presidential candidate evaluations, and voting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 196-210. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.2.196
Kruglansi, A. W. (1975). The Human Subject in the Psychology Experiment: Fact and Artifact. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 101–147.
Kruglanski, A. W. (1980). Lay epistemology process and contents. Psychological Review, 87, 70-87.
Kuhn, R. (2010). President Sarkozy and news media management. French Politics, 8(4), 355-376. DOI: 10.1057/fp.2010.18
Kuklinski, J. H. & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the Rational Public: Cognition, Heuristics, and Mass Opinion. In A. Lupia, M. D., McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of Reason. Cognition, Choice, and the Bounds of Rationality (153-182). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kuklinski, J. H., & Hurley, N. L. (1994). On Hearing and Interpreting Political Messages: A Cautionary Tale of Citizen Cue-Taking. The Journal of Politics, 56(3), 729-751. DOI: 10.2307/2132190
Kunda, Z. (1990). The Case of Motivated Reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480-490. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2008). Reassessing the Role of Anxiety in Vote Choice. Political Psychology, 29(2), 275-296. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00626.x
Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2011). Does Anxiety Improve Voters’ Decision Making? Political Psychology, 32(2), 347-361.
Langer, A. I. (2007). A Historical Exploration of the Personalisation of Politics in the Print Media: The British Prime Ministers (1945-1999). Parliamentary Affairs, 60(3), 371-387. DOI: 10.1093/pa/gsm028
Lau, R. L., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and Disadvantages of Cognitive Heuristics in Political Decision Making. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 951-71. DOI: 10.2307/2669334
Lau, R. L., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How Voters Decide. Information Processing during Election Campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lau, R. R. (2003). Models of Decision-Making. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology (pp. 19-59). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lavine, H. G. (2010). A Sketch of Political Psychology. In H. G. Lavine (Ed.), Political Psychology. London: Sage Publications.
Lavine, H. G., Johnston, Ch. D., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2012). The Ambivalent Partisan. How Critical Loyalty Promotes Democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lavine, H., Bordiga, E., & Sullivan, J. L. (2000). On the Relationship between Attitude Involvement and Attitude Accessibility: Toward a Cognitive-Motivational Model of Political Information Processing. Political Psychology, 21, 81-106. DOI: 10.1111/0162-895X.00178
Lebo, M. J. & Cassino, D. (2007). The aggregated consequences of motivated reasoning and the dynamics of partisan presidential approval. Political Psychology, 28, 723-746.
Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2014). Political Parties, Motivated Reasoning, and Public Opinion Formation. Advances in Political Psychology, 35(1), 129-156.
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, Anger and Risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of Fear and Anger on Perceived Risks of Terrorism: A National Field Experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.01433
Levin, J. R., Serlin, R. C., & Seama, M. A. (1994). A controlled, powerful multiple-comparison strategy for several situations. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 153-159. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.153
Lewis-Beck, M. S. et al. (2008). The American Voter Revisited. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.
Lieberman M. D., Schreiber D., & Ochsner K. N. (2003). Is Political Cognition Like Riding a Bicycle? How Cognitive Neuroscience Can Inform Research on Political Thinking. Political Psychology, 24, 681–704. DOI: 10.1046/j.1467-9221.2003.00347.x
Lieberman, M. D. (2003). Reflective and Reflexive Judgment Processes. A Social Cognitive Neuroscience Approach. In F. P. Joseph, W. D. Kipling, & W. Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes, (pp. 44-67). New York: Cambridge University Press
Lindquist, K. A. (2013). Emotions Emerge from More Basic Psychological Ingredients: A Modern Psychological Constructionist Model. Emotion Review, 5(4), 356-368. DOI: 10.1177/1754073913489750
Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Constructing Emotion. The Experience of Fear as a Conceptual Act. Psychological Science, 19(9), 898-903. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02174.x
Linek, L. (2012). Volební podpora nových stran TOP 09 a VV. In L. Linek (ed.), Voliči a volby 2010 (pp. 166-187). Praha: Sociologické nakladatelství.
Linek, L. (2014). Čistá a celková volatilia v Česku v letech 1990-2013: stejný koncept, odlišná měření a podobné závěry? Acta Politologica, 6(1), 24-38.
Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2005). The Automaticity of Affect for Political Candidates, Parties, and Issues: Experimental Tests of the Hot Cognition Hypothesis. Political Psychology, 26, 455-482. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00426.x
Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2013). The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lodge, M., McGraw, K., & Stroh, P. (1989). An Impression- Driven Model of Candidate Evaluation. American Political Science Review, 83(2), 399-419. DOI: 10.2307/1962397
Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M., & Brau, S. (1995) The Responsive Voter: Campaign Information and the Dynamics of Candidate Evaluation. American Political Science Review, 89, 309-326. DOI: 10.2307/2082427
Lopes, L. L. (1991). The Rhetoric of Irrationality. Theory of Psychology, 1(1), 65-82. DOI: 10.1177/0959354391011005
Lowell, L. A. (1910). The Psychology of Politics. American Political Science Review, 4, 1-15.
Lupia, A. (1994). Shortcuts versus Encyclopedias: Information and Voting Behavior in California Insurance Reform Elections. The American Political Science Review, 88(1), 63-76. DOI: 10.2307/2944882
Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The Democratic Dilemma. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lyons, P. (2008). Politická informovanost: teorie, koncepty a měření. In L. Linek, & P. Lyons (Eds.), Politická informovanost občanů: teorie, měření a role při zkoumání politických postojů (pp. 14- 52). Praha: Sociologický ústav AV ČR.
MacKuen et al. (2010). Civic engagements: resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation. American Journal of Political Science, 54, 440-458. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2010.00440.x
MacKuen, M. (2007). The Third Way: The Theory of Affective Intelligence and American Democracy. In R., Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior (pp. 124-151). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Malhotra, N., & Kuo, A. G. (2008). Attributing Blame: The Public’s Response to Hurricane Katrina. The Journal of Politics, 70(1), 120-135. DOI: 10.1017/S0022381607080097
Malka, A. , & Soto, C. J. (2014) How encompassing is the effect of negativity bias on political conservatism? Behavioral Brain Sicence, 37(3), 320-321. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X13002653
Marcus, G. E., MacKuen, M., & Neuman, R. W. (2011). Parsimony and Complexity: Developing and Testing Theories of Affective Intelligence. Political Psychology, 32(2), 323-336. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00806.x
Marcus, G. E., Neumann, R. W., & MacKuen M. (2000). Affective Intelligence and Political Judgment. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Mark, D. (2009). Going Dirty. The Art of Negative Cmapaigning. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.
Markus, G. B., & Converse, P. E. (1979). A Dynamic Simlutaneous Equation Model of Electoral Choice. American Political Science Review, 3, 1055-1070. DOI: 10.2307/1953989
Mattes, K. & Redlawsk, D. P. (2014). The Positive Case for Negative Campaigns. Chicago: The Chicago University Press.
Matušková, A. (2010). Politický marketing a české politické strany. Volební kampaně 2006. Masarykova univerzita: Brno.
McAllister, I. (2007). The Personalization of Politics. In R. Dalton, & H. D. Klingemann (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Behavior (pp. 571-588). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McDermott, R. (2002). Experimental Methodology in Political Science. Political Analysis, 10, 325-342. DOI: 10.1093/pan/10.4.325
McDermott, R. (2002). Experimental Methods in Political Science. Annual Review of Political Science, 5, 31-61. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.polisci.5.091001.170657
McDermott, R. (2011). Internal and External Validity. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science (pp. 27–40). New York. Cambridge University Press.
McGraw, K. (2000). Contribution of the Cognitive Approach to Political Psychology. Political Psychology, 21 (4), 805-832. DOI: 10.1111/0162-895X.00217
McGraw, K., & Hoekstra, V. M. (1994). Experimentation in Political Science: Historical Trends and Future Directions. In M. Delli Caprini, L. Huddy, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), Research in Micropolitics (pp. 3-30). Greenwich: JAI Press.
McGraw, K., Lodge, M., & Stroh, P. (1990). Order Effects in the Evaluation of Political Candidates. Political Behavior, 12(1), 41-58.
McGuire, W. J. (1969). An information-processing model of advertising effectiveness. In H.L. Davis, & A.J. Silk (Eds.), Behavioral and Management Sciences in Marketing. New York: Ronald.
Mederios, M., & Noel, A. (2002). The Forgotten Side of Partisanship: Negative Party Identification in Four Anglo-American Democracies. Comparative Political Studies, 47(7), 1022-1046. DOI: 10.1177/0010414013488560
Meffert, M. F. et al. (2006). The Effects of Negativity and Motivated Information Processing During Political Campaigns. Journal of Communication, 56, 27-51. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00003.x
Meffert, M. F., & Gschwend T. (2011). Motivated Voters and Campaign Information: Information Selection, Processing, and Effects in a Multiparty System. Paper presented at the ECPR Conference, Reykjavik.
Merolla, J. L., Stephenson, L. B., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2007). La aplicación de los métodos experimentales en el estudio de los atajos informativos en México. Política y Gobierno, 14(1), 117‐142.
Merolla, J. L., Stephenson, L. B., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2008). Can Canadians take a hint? The (in)effectiveness of party labels as information shortcuts in Canada. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 41(3), 673-696. DOI: 10.1017/S0008423908080797
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to Authority: an experimental view. London: Tevistock.
Miller, G. A. (1957). The magic of number seven, plus minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-93. DOI: 10.1037/h0043158
Miller, J. A., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations. Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44 (2), 295-309. DOI: 10.2307/2669312
Milligan, K. et al. (2003). Response Latency Methodology for Survey Research: Measurement and Modeling Strategies. Political Analysis, 11, 289-301. DOI: 10.1093/pan/mpg004
Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Magnnusson, & D. Endler (Eds.), Endler Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology (pp.333-352). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mondak, J. J. (1993). Public Opinion and Heuristic Processing of Source Cues. Political Behavior, 15(2), 167-192. DOI: 10.1007/BF00993852
Mondak, J. J. (1993a). Source Cues and Policy Approval: The Cognitive Dynamics of Public Support for the Reagan Agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 186-212. DOI: 10.2307/2111529
Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the Foundations of Political Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mook, D. G. (1983). In Defense of External Invalidity. American Psychologist, 4, 379–387. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.4.379
Morris, J. P. et al. (2003). Activation of Political Attitudes: A Psychosociological Examination of the Hot Cognition Hypothesis. Political Psychology, 24(4) 727-745. DOI: 10.1046/j.1467-9221.2003.00349.x
Morton, R. B., & Williams, K. C. (2008). Experimentation in Political Science. In J. M. Box-Stefensmeier, H. E. Brady, & D. Collier (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Political Methodology (pp. 339-356). New York: Oxford University Press.
Morton, R. B., & Williams, K. C. (2010). Experimental Political Science and the Study of Causality. From Nature to the Lab. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mulligan, K. et al. (2003). Response Latency Methodology for Survey Research: Measurement and Modeling Strategies. Political Analysis, 11(3), 289-301. DOI: 10.1093/pan/mpg004
Neumann, W. R. et al. (2007). The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Nicholson, S. P. (2011). Dominating Cues and the Limitsof Elite Influence. The Journal of Politics, 73(4), 1165-1177. DOI: 10.1017/S002238161100082X
Nir, L. (2011). Motivated Reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 504-532. DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfq076
Oscarsson, H. (2007). A Matter of Fact? Knowledge Effects on the Vote in Swedish General Elections, 1985-2002. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30(3), 301-322. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9477.2007.00182.x
Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1991). The Rational Public: Fifty Years of Trends in American Policy Preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Payne, J. W. (1982). Contingent decision behavior. Psychology Bulletin, 92, 382-402. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.382
Peters, E., & Slovic, P. (2000). The Springs of Action: Affective and Analytical Information Processing in Choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(12), 1465-1475. DOI: 10.1177/01461672002612002
Petersen, M. B. (2015). Evolutionary Political Psychology: On the Origins and Structure of Heuristics and Biases in Politics. Political Psychology, 36(1), 45-78. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12237
Petersen, M. B. et al. (2013). Motivated Reasoning and Political Parties: Evidence for Increased Processing in the Face of Party Cues. Political Behavior, 35, 831-854. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-012-9213-1
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, T. J. (1979). Issue Involvement Can Increase or Decrease Persuasion by Enhancing Message-Relevant Cognitive Responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1915-1926. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1915
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, T. J. (1981). Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches. Boulder: Westview Press.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, T. J. (1986). Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes of Attitude Change. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate. The Modern Denial of Human Nature. London: Penguin Books.
Poguntke, T., & Webb, P. D. (2005). The Presidentialization of Politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Popkin, S. L. (1991). The Reasoning Voter. Communication and Persuasion in Presidential Campaigns. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1915
Prior, M. (2007). Is Partisan Bias in Perceptions of Objective Conditions Real? Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago.
Rahat, G., & Sheafer, T. (2007). The Personalization(s) of Politics: Israel 1949-2003. Political Communication, 24, 65-80. DOI: 10.1080/10584600601128739
Rahn, W. M. (1993). The Role of Partisan Stereotypes in Information Processing about Political Candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472-96. DOI: 10.2307/2111381
Rahn, W. M. et al. (1990). A social-cognitive model of candidate appraisal. In J. Ferejohn, & J. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and Democratic Processes. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Rahn, W. M., Sullivan, J. L., & Rudolph, T. J. 2002. Political Psychology and political Science. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Thinking about Political Psychology (pp. 155-186). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021-1044. DOI: 10.1111/1468-2508.00161
Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). Feeling Politics. Emotion in Political Information Processing. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Redlawsk, D. P., Civettini, A. J. W., & Lau, R. R. (2007). Affective Intelligence and Voting: Information processing and learning in a campaign. In A. Crigler, M. MacKuen, G. E. Marcus, & W. R. Neuman. (Eds.), The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior (pp. 152-179). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Renshon, J., Lee, J. J., & Tingley, D. (2014). Physiological Arousal and Political Belief. Political Psychology, 36, 569–585. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12173
Riggle, E. D. et al. (1992). Bases of Political Judgments: The Role of Stereotypic and Nonstereotypic Information. Political Behavior, 14(1), 67-87. DOI: 10.1007/BF00993509
Richardson, B. M. (1991). European party loyalties revisited. American Political Science Review, 85: 751-775. DOI: 10.2307/1963849
Rose, R., & Mishler, W. (1998). Negative and positive party identification in post-Communist countries. Electoral Studies, 17, 217-234. DOI: 10.1016/S0261-3794(98)00016-X
Rosema, M. (2006). Partisanship, candidate evaluations, and prospective voting. Electoral Studies, 25(3), 467-488. DOI: 10.1016/j.electstud.2005.06.017
Roth, A. E. (1995). Introduction to Experimental Economics. In J. H. Kagel, & A. E. Roth (Eds.), The Handbook of Experimental Economics (pp. 3-109). Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Rousseau, D. M., & Fried, Y (2001). Location, location, location: Contextualizing organizational research. Journal of Organizational Behavior 22(1):1-13.
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296-320. DOI: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
Russel, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145-172. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145
Samuels, D., & Zucco, C. Jr. (2014). The Power of Partisanship in Brazil: Evidence from Survey Experiments. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 212-225. DOI: 10.1111/ajps.12050
Sarason, I. G., Smith R. E., and Diener E. (1975). Personality research: Components of variance attributable to the person and the situation. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 32(2), 199-204. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.199
Seaman, M. A., Levin, J. R., & Serlin, R. C. (1991). New developments in pairwise multiple comparisons: Some powerful and practical procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 577-586. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.577
Sears, D. O. (1986). College Sophomores in the Laboratory: Influences of a Narrow Data Base on Social Psychology’s View of Human Nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 515-530. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.515
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2001). Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Scherer, K. R. (2005). Unconscious processes in emotion: The bulk of the iceberg. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthall, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and Consciousness (312-334). New York: Guilford.
Schram, A. (2005). Artificiality: The Tension between Internal and External Validity in Economic Experiments. Working paper, University of Amsterdam.
Schulz-Hardt, S. et al. (2000). Biased information search in group decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 655-669. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.655
Sigelman, L. et al. (1991). Reconsidering Pocketbook Voting: An Experimental Approach. Political Behavior, 13 (2), 129-149. DOI: 10.1007/BF00992293
Sikk, A. (2011). Newness as a winning formula for new political parties. Party Politics, 18(4), 465-486. DOI: 10.1177/1354068810389631
Simon, H. A. (1955). A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99-118. DOI: 10.2307/1884852
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational Choice and the Structure of the Environment. Psychological Review, 63(2), 129-138. DOI: 10.1037/h0042769
Simon, H. A. (1965). Administrative Behavior: A Study of Decision-Making Processes in Administration Organization. New York: Free Press.
Simon, H. A. (1978). Rationality as process and as product of thought. American Economic Review, 68, 1-16.
Simon, H.A. (1979). Information processing models of cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 30, 363-396. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ps.30.020179.002051
Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 131-142. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.1.131
Slovic, P. et al. (2002). The Affect Heuristics. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 397-420), New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sniderman, P. M. (2000). Taking Sides. A Fixed Choice Theory of Political Reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality (pp. 67-84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and Choice. Explorations in Political Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Spezio, M. L., & Adolphs, R. (2007). Emotional Processing and Political Judgment: Toward Integrating Political Psychology and Decision Neuroscience. In W. R. Neumann, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), Affect Effect. Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior (pp.71-95). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Stanovich, K. E. (2011). Rationality and the reflective mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Stanyer, J. (2013). Intimate Politics. Publicity, Privacy and the Personal Lives of Politicians in Media-Saturated Democracies. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Stokes, D. E. (1966). Party loyalty and the likelihood of deviating elections. In A. Campbell, P. E. Converse, W. E. Miller, & D. E. Stokes (Eds.), Elections and the Political Order (pp. 125-135), New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2016). The Illusion of Choice in Democratic Politics: The Unconscious Impact of Motivated Political Reasoning. Advances in Political Psychology, 37(1), 61-85.
Taber, C., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Leaders. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755-769. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x
Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and the perseverance of first impressions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 227-236. DOI: 10.2307/3033716
Tetlock, P. E. (1986). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 74-83. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.74
Thomassen, J. (1976). Party identification as a cross-national concept: Its meaning in Netherlands. In I. Budge, I. Crewe, & D. Farlie (Eds.), Party Identification and Beyond: Representations of Voting and Party Competition (pp. 63-80). London: Wiley.
Thomassen, J., & Rosema, M. (2008). Party identification revisited. In J. Bartle, & P. Bellucci (Eds.), Partisanship: Party Identification, Social Identities, and Political Experiences (pp. 42-59). London: Rutledge.
Thurstone, L. L. (1931). Measurement of social attitudes. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 26, 249-269. DOI: 10.1037/h0070363
Tomz, M., & Sniderman, P. M. (2005). Brand Names and the Organization of Mass Belief Systems. Working paper https://web.stanford.edu/~tomz/working/TomzSniderman2005.pdf
Triplett, N. (1989). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. American Journal of Psychology, 9 (4), 507-33. DOI: 10.2307/1412188
Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory Data Analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Turner, J. F. (2007). The Messenger Overwhelming the Message: The Impact of Ideological Cues on Informational Acquisition, Counterarguing and Perceptions of Media Bias. Ph.D. Dissertation, Florida State University.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185 (4157), 1124-1131. DOI: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
Tyler, S. W., Hertel, P. T., McCallum M. C., & Ilis, H. C. (1979). Cognitive effort and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 607-617.
Vaish, A., Grossman, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383-403. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383
Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., Groenendyk, E. W., Gregorowicz, K., & Hutchings, V. L. (2011). Election Night’s Alright for Fighting: The Role of Emotions in Political Participation. The Journal of Politics, 73(2), 156-170. DOI: 10.1017/S0022381610000939
Valentino, N. A., Gregorowicz, K., & Groenendyk, E. W. (2009). Efficacy, Emotions and the Habit of Participation. Political Behavior, 31, 307-330. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-008-9076-7
Van Aelst, P., Sheafer, T., & Stanyer, J. (2011). The personalization of mediated political communication: A review of concepts, operationalizations and key findings. Journalism, 13(2), 1-18.
Van Bavel, J. J. et al. 2016. Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. PNAS. Available at: http://www.psych.nyu.edu/vanbavel/lab/documents/VanBavel.et.al.Contextual.Sensitivity.PNAS.pdf
Van Houweling, R. P., & Sniderman, P. M. (2005). The Political Logic of Downsian Space. Working paper, University of California Berkley.
Vlachová, K. (2003). Dynamika pozitivní a negativní stranické identifikace v České republice. Sociologický časopis, 39(4), 487-508.
Von Neumann, J., & Morgernstern, O. (1994). Theory of Games and Economics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Voženílková, M., & Šedo, J. (2015). Personalizace volebního systému v návrzích volebních reforem v České republice. Evropská volební studia, 10(1), 33-50.
Walster, E., Aronson, E., & Abrahams, D. (1996). On increasing the persuasiveness of a low prestige communicator. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(4), 325–34. DOI: 10.1016/0022-1031(66)90026-6
Wattenberg, M. P. (1995). The Rise of Candidate-Centered Politics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Westen, D., et al. (2006). Neural Bases of Motivated Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Emotional Constraints on Partisan Political Judgment in the 2004 U.S. Presidential Election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(11), 1947-1958.
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and Thinking. Preferences Need No Inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151-175. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the Primacy of Affect. American Psychologist, 39(2), 117-123. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.117
Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, H. (1982). Affective and cognitive factors in preferences. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 123-131. DOI: 10.1086/208905
Zaller, J. (1992). The Nature and Origins of Public Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A Simple Theory of Survey Response: Answering Questions versus Revealing Preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 93-108. DOI: 10.2307/2111583